全文获取类型
收费全文 | 299篇 |
免费 | 31篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
337篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 1篇 |
2024年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 10篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 9篇 |
2015年 | 11篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 60篇 |
2012年 | 9篇 |
2011年 | 6篇 |
2010年 | 2篇 |
2009年 | 5篇 |
2008年 | 17篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 12篇 |
2005年 | 15篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 13篇 |
2001年 | 15篇 |
2000年 | 7篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有337条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
201.
中国的行为遗传学诞生在遗传学突飞猛进的时代,令人振奋。作为一个新兴学科,中国的行为遗传学研究可以避免走前人的弯路,而成为领先而不是滞后的领域。以学习能力的研究为例,计量行为遗传学可以避免“性与养”(nature vs. nurture)的争论而直接研究一些遗传学与心理学里有意义的问题,包括正常与异常、稳定与变动、同质与异质的关系等问题。尤为重要的是超越“性与养”的争论而直接研究遗传对行为的影响。本文也扼要介绍了英国“双生子早期发展”项目的计量行为遗传学和“全基因组关联”对学习能力研究的结果。可以预计,飞速发展的遗传学发现将会持续一些时日,其发展将会对中国和世界的心理学产生更大的影响 相似文献
202.
商卫星 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2007,28(1A):5-7,10
随着现代科技的进步,脑科学特别是脑成像技术能够无损伤地探测人脑,成为脑功能研究的“显微镜”。这些科技的发展对心理学研究具有重大影响。它使得意识能够受到真正科学地研究,促使心理学研究从“计算机隐喻”向“脑隐喻”的转变,并将使心理学理论形态更加复杂。 相似文献
203.
Leary的人际理论将人际互动行为划分为\"情感\"和\"控制\"两个维度,互动双方在情感维度的一致性和控制维度的互补性统称为人际互补。为探究心理咨询过程中人际互补的发展趋势及其与工作同盟、咨询效果的关系,采用电脑操纵杆方法,编制《咨询会谈人际互补行为评分手册》,对16个个案的48个心理咨询会谈录像进行评估。结果表明:有经验的咨询师在咨询前期的情感轴一致性高于中期和后期,在咨询后期的控制轴互补性高于前期和中期,新手咨询师的情感轴一致性和控制轴互补性均无阶段性差异;咨询中期的情感轴一致性负向预测工作同盟和会谈深度;情感轴一致性呈高-低-高发展趋势的个案咨询效果更好。结论:心理咨询的人际互补三阶段模型得到一定程度的支持,其作用机制有待进一步验证。 相似文献
204.
Pernille Bruhn 《Advances in cognitive psychology / University of Finance and Management in Warsaw》2013,9(2):62-73
In this article, we present a novel experimental approach to the study of
anticipation in probabilistic cuing. We implemented a modified spatial cuing
task in which participants made an anticipatory hand movement toward one of two
probabilistic targets while the (x,
y)-computer mouse coordinates of their hand movements were
sampled. This approach allowed us to tap into anticipatory processes as they
occurred, rather than just measuring their behavioral outcome through reaction
time to the target. In different conditions, we varied the participants’ degree
of certainty of the upcoming target position with probabilistic pre-cues. We
found that participants initiated spontaneous anticipatory hand movements in all
conditions, even when they had no information on the position of the upcoming
target. However, participants’ hand position immediately before the target was
affected by the degree of certainty concerning the target’s position. This
modulation of anticipatory hand movements emerged rapidly in most participants
as they encountered a constant probabilistic relation between a cue and an
upcoming target position over the course of the experiment. Finally, we found
individual differences in the way anticipatory behavior was modulated with an
uncertain/neutral cue. Implications of these findings for probabilistic spatial
cuing are discussed. 相似文献
205.
ABSTRACT— One major contribution of neuroscience to understanding cognitive development has been in demonstrating that biology is not destiny—that is, demonstrating the remarkable role of experience in shaping the mind, brain, and body. Only rarely has neuroscience provided wholly new insights into cognitive development, but often it has provided evidence of mechanisms by which observations of developmental psychologists could be explained. Behavioral findings have often remained controversial until an underlying biological mechanism for them was offered. Neuroscience has demonstrated promise for detecting cognitive problems before they are behaviorally observable—and, hence, promise for early intervention. In this article, we discuss examples drawn from imitation and mirror neurons, phenylketonuria (PKU) and prefrontal dopamine, maternal touch and stress reactivity, and nongenetic (behavioral) intergenerational transmission of biological characteristics. 相似文献
206.
《Child & family behavior therapy》2013,35(1):43-60
Behavioral analyses of 3 adolescents show that 3 months of targeted family problem-solving training can decrease drug use and school failure by the end of a 1 1/4 year follow-up while control behaviors remain stable. Some findings, however, reflected incomplete understanding of the controlling variables: (a) lengthy delays before behavioral improvement, (b) recurrences of the problem behavior and subsequent recoveries during follow-up, and (c) correlated changes in the home and school environments. It is suggested that systematic study of relevant variables in the intervention could reduce behavioral variability and further increase understanding of adolescent drug use. 相似文献
207.
208.
209.
210.
In theoretical analyses of early mother–child interaction, it has been argued that interaction should be studied in its flow over time, and that the behaviour of each interactant is likely to be non‐linearly determined. The mother–child dyad can be seen as a non‐linear dynamic system, the development of which is determined by the mutual relations between its elements. The present study is based on the idea that computer simulations can be used to find out what kind of empirical implication these ideas have. Accordingly, we describe two non‐linear dynamic systems‐based models for simulating mother–child interaction, i.e. a connectionist network model and a logistic growth model. Three determinants of the nature of the interaction, i.e. the child's irritability, the mother's sensitive responsiveness, and the intensity of an external stressor bothering the child, are varied systematically. Although the results of both simulations differed considerably, they shared the fact that small changes in stressor intensity produced abrupt changes from one type of interaction to another. In addition, increasing stressor intensity sometimes had the paradoxical effect of resulting in less, rather than more, distress on the side of the child. Though irritability and responsiveness were varied in a less fine‐grained way than stressor intensity, the results suggest that similarly small differences in these dimensions at different parts of the dimension's scale range have differentially strong effects on the nature of the interaction. It is concluded that these simulations help us to specify the nature of empirically researchable phenomena that are to be expected, given the assumptions listed above. Further elaboration of the models and comparison with longitudinal empirical data is needed to answer further theoretical and practical questions. Copyright © 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献