全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5286篇 |
免费 | 606篇 |
国内免费 | 199篇 |
专业分类
6091篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 12篇 |
2024年 | 67篇 |
2023年 | 84篇 |
2022年 | 95篇 |
2021年 | 178篇 |
2020年 | 217篇 |
2019年 | 300篇 |
2018年 | 335篇 |
2017年 | 391篇 |
2016年 | 357篇 |
2015年 | 165篇 |
2014年 | 222篇 |
2013年 | 1237篇 |
2012年 | 138篇 |
2011年 | 259篇 |
2010年 | 166篇 |
2009年 | 221篇 |
2008年 | 224篇 |
2007年 | 216篇 |
2006年 | 174篇 |
2005年 | 180篇 |
2004年 | 159篇 |
2003年 | 127篇 |
2002年 | 97篇 |
2001年 | 83篇 |
2000年 | 43篇 |
1999年 | 55篇 |
1998年 | 46篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 28篇 |
1993年 | 21篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有6091条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
The hypothesis that borderline personality disorder (BPD) is related to overgeneral memories was tested in a mixed sample of 39 patients. A memory test with emotional cue words and the instruction to produce specific autobiographical memories was used. Specificity was judged by an independent rater. Regression analyses indicated that age and major depressive disorder were related to the production of less specific memories, whereas educational level and presence of personality disorder were positivily related to number of specific memories. Borderline personality disorder, anxiety disorders and childhood traumas were not related to number of specific memories. 相似文献
72.
A Hebrew adaptation of Gardner and Brownell's (1986) "Right Hemisphere Communication Battery" (HRHCB) was administered to 27 right brain-damaged (RBD) patients, 31 left brain-damaged (LBD) patients, and 21 age-matched normal controls. Both patient groups showed deficits relative to controls and overall there was no difference between the two patient groups. A factor analysis of patients' scores on the HRHCB yielded two interpretable factors, a verbal and a nonverbal one. These factors were not lateralized. Performance of patients on the HRHCB correlated significantly and positively with performance on most tests of basic language functions, measured with a Hebrew adaptation of the "Western Aphasia Battery" (HWAB) and with other cognitive functions measured with standardized neuropsychological tests. There were stronger correlations of HRHCB with subtests of the HWAB in LBD patients and with nonlanguage cognitive tests in RBD patients. In the LBD group, HRHCB subtests' scores correlated negatively with lesion extent in frontal and temporal perisylvian regions. Such localization was not observed in RBD patients. The results argue against selective right hemisphere (RH) involvement in the RHCB, alleged to measure pragmatic aspects of language use, and show, instead, bilateral involvement. The results also argue against a modular organization of these functions of language use, especially in the RH. 相似文献
73.
An adult California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) with extensive experience in performing discrimination learning tasks was tested to evaluate her long-term memory for two
previously learned concepts. An associative concept, that of equivalence classification, was retested after a retention interval
of approximately 1 year. The sea lion had originally shown emergent equivalence classification with nonsimilarity-based classes
of stimuli in a simple discrimination repeated-reversal procedure as well as in a matching-to-sample procedure. The 1-year
memory test revealed no decrement in classification performance in either procedure. A relational concept, that of generalized
identity matching, was retested after approximately 10 years. The sea lion had originally received trial-and-error exemplar
training with identity matching-to-sample problems prior to transferring the concept to novel stimulus configurations. In
the 10-year memory test, the sea lion immediately and reliably applied the previously established identity concept to familiar
and novel sets of matching problems. These are the first reports of long-term conceptual memory in a nonprimate species. The
experimental findings are consistent with a variety of observations of sea lions in natural settings, which indicate that
natal sites, feeding areas, and individuals may be remembered over long periods of time. Electronic Supplementary Material is available if you access this article at http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-002-0153-8. On that page (frame on the left side),
a link takes you directly to the supplementary material.
Electronic Publication 相似文献
74.
This study examined the role of global processing speed in mediating age increases in auditory memory span in 5- to 13-year-olds. Children were tested on measures of memory span, processing speed, single-word speech rate, phonological sensitivity, and vocabulary. Structural equation modeling supported a model in which age-associated increases in processing speed predicted the availability of long-term memory phonological representations for redintegration processes. The availability of long-term phonological representations, in turn, explained variance in memory span. Maximum speech rate did not predict independent variance in memory span. 相似文献
75.
When participants search for a target letter while reading, they make more omissions if the target letter is embedded in frequent function words than in less frequent content words. Reflecting developmental changes in component language and literacy skills, the size of this effect increases with age. With adults, the missing-letter effect is due to both word function and word frequency. With children, it is unclear whether the growing size of the missing-letter effect across development is due to a larger effect of word function, word frequency, or both because previous studies with children seeking to isolate the influence of word frequency and word function suffer from important methodological limitations. With these methodological limitations eliminated (Experiments 1 and 2), performance in a letter detection task was assessed for children in Grades 1, 2, 3, 4, and 7 as well as for undergraduate students. The results revealed that the influence of word function increases with age, whereas the effect of frequency is fairly stable across ages. Furthermore, normative predictability data collected in Experiment 3 revealed that third graders and undergraduate students were equally good at predicting function slots in a sentence. 相似文献
76.
This review identified 1275 studies examining cognitive deficits in people with schizophrenia, published between 1990 and
2003. Data from 113 studies (4365 patients and 3429 controls) were combined in a meta-analysis carried out on the five cognitive
domains of IQ, memory, language, executive function, and attention. Studies were excluded where they lacked a suitable control
group or failed to present complete information. In all five cognitive domains, analysis indicated a consistent trend for
patients to perform more poorly than healthy controls, with significant heterogeneity across studies. Sources of heterogeneity
were analyzed and a need to ensure more appropriate composition of patient and control groups and to adopt a more refined
and methodologically correct, hypothesis-driven approach was identified. 相似文献
77.
Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: Contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathematical disability 总被引:19,自引:0,他引:19
Geary DC Hoard MK Byrd-Craven J DeSoto MC 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2004,88(2):121-151
Groups of first-grade (mean age = 82 months), third-grade (mean age = 107 months), and fifth-grade (mean age = 131 months) children with a learning disability in mathematics (MD, n = 58) and their normally achieving peers (n = 91) were administered tasks that assessed their knowledge of counting principles, working memory, and the strategies used to solve simple (4+3) and complex (16+8) addition problems. In all grades, the children with MD showed a working memory deficit, and in first grade, the children with MD used less sophisticated strategies and committed more errors while solving simple and complex addition problems. The group differences in strategy usage and accuracy were related, in part, to the group difference in working memory and to group and individual differences in counting knowledge. Across grade-level and group, the switch from simple to complex addition problems resulted in a shift in the mix of problem-solving strategies. Individual differences in the strategy mix and in the strategy shift were related, in part, to individual differences in working memory capacity and counting knowledge. 相似文献
78.
Two studies were conducted to examine developmental progression in children's and adults' ability to adequately monitor their own attempts to recall event details as well as the dependence of such metamemorial competencies on question formats. Eight and 10-year-old children as well as adults (Study 1, N=116; Study 2, N=60) rated their confidence when responding to specific questions about an observed event. Confirming most recent results, children and adults gave higher confidence ratings after correct than after incorrect answers. This ability, however, was limited to an unbiased question format. When being asked misleading questions, children's ability to differentiate was undermined, as reflected in equally high confidence judgments after correct and incorrect answers, even when the interview contained a mix of misleading and unbiased questions. When the interviewer "bombarded" the children with an uninterrupted series of misleading questions, children's difficulties appeared to be even more pronounced. These findings highlight the importance of the way in which questions are asked, and point to age-related progression in the relative impact of questioning style. 相似文献
79.
Multiple measures of three of the factors (capacity, strategies, and metamemory) hypothesized to cause improvements in memory with age were obtained from 179 children in kindergarten to second grade (younger: ages 5-8) or third and fourth grade (older: ages 8-11) during nine sessions of testing. Confirmatory factor analysis was computed separately for each age group. Results suggested that the fit of the three-factor model was statistically significantly better than a one-factor, general memory model for both age groups. However, the fit indices were borderline, and there was not sufficient evidence for a metamemory factor for younger children. The factors that influence memory performance may differ with age. 相似文献