全文获取类型
收费全文 | 10861篇 |
免费 | 1115篇 |
国内免费 | 319篇 |
专业分类
12295篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 31篇 |
2024年 | 157篇 |
2023年 | 260篇 |
2022年 | 229篇 |
2021年 | 282篇 |
2020年 | 539篇 |
2019年 | 523篇 |
2018年 | 452篇 |
2017年 | 494篇 |
2016年 | 467篇 |
2015年 | 368篇 |
2014年 | 448篇 |
2013年 | 1256篇 |
2012年 | 294篇 |
2011年 | 399篇 |
2010年 | 346篇 |
2009年 | 446篇 |
2008年 | 512篇 |
2007年 | 525篇 |
2006年 | 537篇 |
2005年 | 475篇 |
2004年 | 413篇 |
2003年 | 319篇 |
2002年 | 271篇 |
2001年 | 192篇 |
2000年 | 153篇 |
1999年 | 156篇 |
1998年 | 126篇 |
1997年 | 106篇 |
1996年 | 82篇 |
1995年 | 85篇 |
1994年 | 64篇 |
1993年 | 62篇 |
1992年 | 57篇 |
1990年 | 40篇 |
1989年 | 41篇 |
1988年 | 28篇 |
1987年 | 29篇 |
1985年 | 135篇 |
1984年 | 132篇 |
1983年 | 114篇 |
1982年 | 93篇 |
1981年 | 69篇 |
1980年 | 83篇 |
1979年 | 70篇 |
1978年 | 105篇 |
1977年 | 61篇 |
1976年 | 49篇 |
1975年 | 31篇 |
1973年 | 28篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
931.
Memory recall has been proposed as a common and effective mood regulation strategy. Although several studies have presented results suggesting that recalling valenced memories affects subsequent mood, their designs allow for alternative interpretations of the observed effects. Two such alternatives include the reverse effect (mood effects on memory due to non-experimental assignment to memory recall condition) and demand characteristics of the experiment. We used covert experimental assignment to memory condition, asking subjects (N=314; 56% female) to recall memories that were primarily positive, neutral, or negative. Results showed the expected effect on mood (p<.002), with reported mood worst in the negative memory condition, better in the neutral condition, and best in the positive condition. These results suggest that valenced memory recall does indeed exert an effect on mood, and may do so even without the individual's awareness. 相似文献
932.
Sixty-one participants from the community participated in a randomised controlled trial of group debriefing to assess the effect of this intervention upon memory for a stressful event. Participants were randomly allocated to one of three groups: debriefing; debriefing with an experimenter confederate present (who supplied three pieces of misinformation to the group regarding the stressful event); and a no-treatment control. All groups were shown a very stressful video and were again reviewed after 1 month. Members of the debriefing group where a confederate provided misinformation were more likely to recall this misinformation as fact than members of the other two groups. The debriefing group was also more accurate in their recall of peripheral content than the confederate group. Across all groups, participants were found to be more accurate at central rather than peripheral recall yet more confident for incorrect memories of the video than correct memories. 相似文献
933.
著名心理学家W ard Edwards对行为决策的学科形成与理论发展作出了巨大贡献。文章简要介绍了Edwards的生平,以主观期望效用模型和贝叶斯决策为核心回顾了Edwards的决策理论、主要成就及其对行为决策研究的学术贡献。文章并从行为决策领域的研究、科普、应用及人才培养等方面总结了中国行为决策研究的发展历程,以及Edwards的学术思想对中国行为决策研究的主要影响。文章展望了行为决策研究的前景,并呼吁更多的中国心理学工作者投身于行为决策研究。 相似文献
934.
人与自然关系是文学表现的重要母题。透过文学的发展,可以窥视出人类社会在社会实践推动下所走过的历史道路。本文意在通过中西文学发展中人与自然关系的描写,揭示人与自然关系的变化过程,并在历史的视野中审视生态批判的当代意义。 相似文献
935.
Matthias Adam 《Synthese》2007,158(1):93-108
Scientific claims can be assessed epistemically in either of two ways: according to scientific standards, or by means of philosophical
arguments such as the no-miracle argument in favor of scientific realism. This paper investigates the basis of this duality
of epistemic assessments. It is claimed that the duality rests on two different notions of epistemic justification that are
well-known from the debate on internalism and externalism in general epistemology: a deontological and an alethic notion.
By discussing the conditions for the scientific acceptability of empirical results, it is argued that intrascientific justification
employs the deontological notion. Philosophical disputes such as those on scientific realism can by contrast be shown to rest
on the alethic notion. The implications of these findings both for the nature of the respective epistemic projects and for
their interrelation are explored. 相似文献
936.
937.
938.
Lesions of the basal ganglia, thalamus, and deep white matter: Differential effects on language functions 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Claus-W. Wallesch Hans H. Kornhuber Richard J. Brunner Thomas Kunz Beate Hollerbach Gebhard Suger 《Brain and language》1983,20(2):286-304
Forty-five patients with unilateral demarcated vascular lesions in the basal ganglia, the thalamus and the deep white matter were investigated with an "aphasia battery." Patients with basal ganglia lesions performed worse than both other groups in tests of articulation, syntax, and lexical functions. The deficit of patients with basal ganglia lesions on all expressive language modalities was lateralized to the left hemisphere. Patients with left thalamic lesions showed impairments of speech fluency and in the Token Test. Patients with white matter lesions alone showed no effect of laterality in tests of language functions. The results are discussed on the basis of a recent theory of the participation of the deep nuclei in language processing. 相似文献
939.
Written spelling deficit of Broca''s aphasics 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The purpose of this study was to investigate the nature of the written spelling deficit manifested by Broca's aphasics. Four spelling tests were given to eight Broca's aphasic patients. Analysis of misspelling errors led the investigators to conclude that Broca's aphasics do not spell phonically, but rather adhere exclusively to a visual/orthographic strategy. Possible cognitive deficits underlying the spelling problem are identified. The written spelling deficit is related to other features of the syndrome of Broca's aphasia, and finally, speculations are offered concerning the neurological substrate of written spelling in Broca's aphasic patients. 相似文献
940.
"八荣八耻"的道德哲学 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
“八荣八耻”的社会主义荣辱观,把社会主义道德的要求聚焦到荣辱观,集中点击人的良心,可谓四两拨千斤;“八荣八耻”督促人们作出善恶选择,启迪了个人的道德自觉、自主和自律,同时也强调了公民的道德权利和义务;“八荣八耻”的落实重在修养和践行,为社会主义道德建设与和谐社会的构建创造了精神条件。 相似文献