首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   18586篇
  免费   1769篇
  国内免费   1143篇
  2023年   242篇
  2022年   228篇
  2021年   333篇
  2020年   634篇
  2019年   721篇
  2018年   678篇
  2017年   811篇
  2016年   791篇
  2015年   531篇
  2014年   636篇
  2013年   2248篇
  2012年   417篇
  2011年   553篇
  2010年   436篇
  2009年   651篇
  2008年   816篇
  2007年   866篇
  2006年   796篇
  2005年   678篇
  2004年   599篇
  2003年   491篇
  2002年   458篇
  2001年   287篇
  2000年   270篇
  1999年   224篇
  1998年   190篇
  1997年   158篇
  1996年   128篇
  1995年   120篇
  1994年   109篇
  1993年   99篇
  1992年   71篇
  1991年   58篇
  1990年   54篇
  1989年   39篇
  1988年   46篇
  1986年   34篇
  1985年   383篇
  1984年   431篇
  1983年   351篇
  1982年   458篇
  1981年   455篇
  1980年   456篇
  1979年   410篇
  1978年   459篇
  1977年   374篇
  1976年   367篇
  1975年   280篇
  1974年   278篇
  1973年   236篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
Three measures of chronic self-esteem, plus four measures developed to assess situational components of self-esteem (task-specific self-esteem and social self-esteem), were administered to 238 undergraduates at a large, urban university and a community college in the southeastern United States. Evidence for a composite construct of self-esteem was found. Results also indicate task-specific self-esteem correlates as high as .42 (p < .01) with chronic self-esteem and as high as .76 (p < .01) with social self-esteem. Task-specific self-esteem was found to be a better predictor of grade point average than chronic or social self-esteem. Suggestions are made for further research on the situational components of self-esteem to extend current findings.  相似文献   
122.
Transfer of matching-to-figure samples in the pigeon   总被引:2,自引:1,他引:1       下载免费PDF全文
Three pigeons were trained on a modified six-key matching-to-sample procedure. The third peck on the figure-sample key (which presented a bird, hand, face, beetle, rabbit, fish, flower, or red hue, as the sample) lighted only one comparison key. Every three additional pecks on the sample lighted another comparison key, up to a maximum of five keys. Pecks on keys of matching figures produced grain. Pecks on nonmatching keys (mismatches) turned off all lights on the comparison keys and repeated the trial. Three figures were used during acquisition. The birds learned to peck each sample until the matching comparison stimulus appeared on one of three comparison stimulus keys, and then to peck that key. Later, five novel stimuli, employed as both sample and comparison stimuli, and two additional matching keys were added. Each bird showed matching transfer to the novel samples. The data suggest that the birds may have learned the concept of figure matching rather than a series of two-component chains or discrete five-key discriminations.  相似文献   
123.
Rats' lever pressing produced sucrose reinforcers on a variable-interval schedule where, in different conditions, the duration of a stimulus presented immediately after reinforcement was either correlated or uncorrelated with the duration of the current interreinforcement interval. Under the baseline schedule, in which no stimulus was presented, the minimum interreinforcement interval was 8 s and the mean postreinforcement pause of each subject approximated this value. Response rates increased slowly over the first 10 to 15 s and then remained roughly constant throughout the remainder of the interval. In both the correlated and uncorrelated conditions, the added stimulus resulted in the postreinforcement pauses lengthening to values in excess of the duration of the preceding stimulus. This resulted in a poststimulus pause which was, in most cases, roughly constant irrespective of the duration of the preceding stimulus, or of the reinforcement contingencies prevailing immediately after stimulus offset. Local response-rate patterns in the uncorrelated conditions were similar to those obtained under the baseline schedule in which no stimulus was presented. However, in the correlated condition local response rates increased across the remainder of the interreinforcer interval. Further, the rate of acceleration was inversely related to the duration of the preceding stimulus. These results show that a correlation between stimulus duration and the ensuing time to reinforcement can control behavior—a type of temporal control not previously reported.  相似文献   
124.
125.
126.
127.
Behavioral conceptions of alcohol abuse often include the hypothesis that drinking behavior is a negatively reinforced operant, with ethanol intoxication viewed as alleviating aversive environmental and internal states. This hypothesis has not been confirmed or refuted by previous studies which employed mild stressors and limited assessment methodology. In the present experiment, 22 patients with severe phobias approached their phobic animal under two consecutive conditions—first while sober and second after drinking either a placebo or an intoxicating dose of ethanol. The severe anxiety induced was assessed behaviorally, physiologically and by the patient's self-report of fear. The intoxicated patients did not experience decreased anxiety, tachycardia or avoidance, compared to the placebo group. These results have clinical implications and suggest the need to reconsider tension-reduction theories of alcohol abuse.  相似文献   
128.
129.
130.
Fear and courage     
S. Rachman   《Behavior Therapy》1984,15(1):109-120
  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号