全文获取类型
收费全文 | 262篇 |
免费 | 45篇 |
国内免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
322篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 1篇 |
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 13篇 |
2022年 | 10篇 |
2021年 | 20篇 |
2020年 | 24篇 |
2019年 | 29篇 |
2018年 | 15篇 |
2017年 | 15篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 68篇 |
2012年 | 7篇 |
2011年 | 14篇 |
2010年 | 9篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 9篇 |
2007年 | 7篇 |
2006年 | 4篇 |
2005年 | 8篇 |
2004年 | 3篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 1篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有322条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
281.
《Journal of Cognitive Psychology》2013,25(3):339-364
The aim of the present study is to uncover the relation between cognitive ability and the answer patterns yielded by the truth table task. According to the Classical Mental Models Theory, people with high working memory capacity answer according to two-valued or “logical” answer patterns. The Suppositional Theory and the Revised Mental Models Theory predict that the answer patterns given by the most intelligent ones are three-valued or “defective”. Correlations are examined, and in three experiments it is tested with a dual task paradigm whether a differential working memory load alters participants' answer patterns. A positive correlation is observed between cognitive ability and three-valued answer patterns, but no effect of the working memory load manipulation is revealed. With an inspection of the classification times we shed light on the processes underlying truth table judgements. We conclude that the Revised Mental Models Theory provides the best account for our results. 相似文献
282.
In vision, it is well established that the perceptual load of a relevant task determines the extent to which irrelevant distractors are processed. Much less research has addressed the effects of perceptual load within hearing. Here, we provide an extensive test using two different perceptual load manipulations, measuring distractor processing through response competition and awareness report. Across four experiments, we consistently failed to find support for the role of perceptual load in auditory selective attention. We therefore propose that the auditory system – although able to selectively focus processing on a relevant stream of sounds – is likely to have surplus capacity to process auditory information from other streams, regardless of the perceptual load in the attended stream. This accords well with the notion of the auditory modality acting as an ‘early-warning’ system as detection of changes in the auditory scene is crucial even when the perceptual demands of the relevant task are high. 相似文献
283.
Joel E. Milam Jean L. Richardson Gary Marks Carol A. Kemper Allen J. Mccutchan 《Psychology & health》2013,28(2):167-181
Relationships between dispositional optimism and pessimism and the course of HIV infection, determined by changes in viral load and CD4 counts, were studied in a longitudinal cohort of 412 patients on antiretroviral therapy (ART). Multiple regression analyses controlling for baseline levels of disease status, ethnicity, and depressive symptoms demonstrated that higher pessimism at baseline was associated with higher viral load at follow-up (average of 18 months later). Optimism at baseline had a curvilinear relationship with CD4 counts at follow-up. Moderate levels of optimism at baseline predicted the highest CD4 counts at follow-up. Although optimism and pessimism were associated with specific health behaviors (e.g., ART adherence, cigarette use, drug use, dietary practices), none of these behaviors mediated the optimism/pessimism effects. The biologic and behavioral mediators of associations of personality variables with the course of treated HIV infection deserve continued investigation. 相似文献
284.
Memmert (2010) tried to foster the development of attentional research by discussing four differences between attentional misdirection (AM) and inattentional blindness (IB). Considering this goal, the comment was received in the intended way by the comments of (Most, 2010) and (Moran and Brady, 2010) who make a number of highly valuable suggestions for further progress. As initially suggested by Memmert (2010) this dialog should help unravel the underlying attentional mechanisms of different paradigms. Therefore, we first discuss the suggested distinction between central and spatial IB by Most (2010). Second, we argue that working memory and perceptual load research seem particularly interesting in this regard and should be taken into consideration when conducting future research along the lines of IB and AM. Third, representative task designs can be an important mosaic piece in across-the-board attention theories and highly useful for deriving further testable hypothesis in naturalistic settings. The most important claim of all commentaries in this issue is that the proposed ideas can all be empirically tested and thereby contribute to the advancement of an unified theoretical framework incorporating IB, AM in consideration of overt and covert attention mechanisms. 相似文献
285.
The main purpose of this paper is to investigate the sensitivity analysis of structural equation model when minor perturbation
is introduced. Some influence measure that based on the general case weight perturbation is derived for the generalized least
squares estimation. An influence measure that related to the Cook's distance is also developed for the special case deletion
perturbation scheme. Using the proposed methodology, the influential observation in a data set can be detected. Moreover,
the general theory can be applied to detect the influential parameters in a model. Finally, some illustrative artificial and
real examples are presented.
The research of the first author was supported by a Hong Kong UPGC grant. The authors are greatly indebted to two reviewers
for some very valuable comments for improvement of the paper. 相似文献
286.
287.
Psychosocial work factors, physical work load and associated musculoskeletal symptoms among home care workers 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
JAN Å JOHANSSON 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》1995,36(2):113-129
This study was based on a questionnaire and included a group of home care workers (HCW) (n = 305) and a reference group of municipal employees (n = 694). The relationship between the work environment and musculoskeletal symptoms was analysed. The HCW were less satisfied with their control over their work and stimulus from their work and had a higher physical work load and prevalence of musculoskeletal symptoms, compared with the reference group. The Rate Ratio (RR) of neck and shoulder symptoms among HCW was 83 and 54 %, respectively, higher among those reporting a "high" psychological work load compared with those reporting a "low" load. The highest RR for a single risk indicator was 2.5, and this concerned low-back symptoms among HCW who often worked with twisted postures. A combination of "poor" psychosocial work environment and "high" physical work load produced the highest RR for work-related neck (RR = 2.57) and shoulder (RR = 2.13) symptoms. 相似文献
288.
289.
Siyuan Chen Julien Epps Fred Paas 《The British journal of educational psychology》2023,93(Z2):318-338
290.