全文获取类型
收费全文 | 9767篇 |
免费 | 845篇 |
国内免费 | 632篇 |
专业分类
11244篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 40篇 |
2023年 | 216篇 |
2022年 | 165篇 |
2021年 | 177篇 |
2020年 | 434篇 |
2019年 | 429篇 |
2018年 | 385篇 |
2017年 | 427篇 |
2016年 | 358篇 |
2015年 | 275篇 |
2014年 | 376篇 |
2013年 | 1068篇 |
2012年 | 221篇 |
2011年 | 351篇 |
2010年 | 323篇 |
2009年 | 387篇 |
2008年 | 488篇 |
2007年 | 549篇 |
2006年 | 554篇 |
2005年 | 467篇 |
2004年 | 437篇 |
2003年 | 328篇 |
2002年 | 276篇 |
2001年 | 176篇 |
2000年 | 136篇 |
1999年 | 135篇 |
1998年 | 109篇 |
1997年 | 85篇 |
1996年 | 63篇 |
1995年 | 62篇 |
1994年 | 49篇 |
1993年 | 51篇 |
1992年 | 36篇 |
1991年 | 23篇 |
1990年 | 24篇 |
1989年 | 29篇 |
1987年 | 19篇 |
1985年 | 162篇 |
1984年 | 167篇 |
1983年 | 135篇 |
1982年 | 129篇 |
1981年 | 108篇 |
1980年 | 113篇 |
1979年 | 106篇 |
1978年 | 155篇 |
1977年 | 103篇 |
1976年 | 99篇 |
1975年 | 79篇 |
1974年 | 72篇 |
1973年 | 69篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
Gerald Sagotsky Charlotte J Patterson Mark R Lepper 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1978,25(2):242-253
A field experiment investigated the effects of training in self-monitoring and goal-setting skills on classroom study behavior and academic achievement among elementary school students in an individualized mathematics program. In the Self-Monitoring Conditions, students were shown a simple system for observing and maintaining daily records of their own study behavior during their math classes; orthogonally, in the Goal-Setting Conditions, students were shown a simple method of setting and recording daily performance goals during their math classes. Exposure to self-monitoring procedures produced significant increases in both appropriate study behavior and in actual achievement in the mathematics program, while exposure to goal-setting procedures had no effect on either study behavior or academic achievement. Nor, contrary to expectation, did exposure to the goal-setting instructions enhance the effectiveness of the self-monitoring system. The implications of these results in terms of implicit and explicit models of self-reinforcement processes were discussed. 相似文献
92.
Second- and sixth-grade (ages 7 and 11 years, respectively) subjects were instructed to learn simple Spanish vocabulary nouns using the keyword method. To remember a foreign word translation the keyword method user must: (1) associate the foreign word to an English word (the keyword) that sounds like part of the foreign word; and (2) remember an image of the keyword and translation referents interacting. Second-grade keyword users who were provided with interactive pictures remembered more vocabulary items than those who generated their own imagery links when given separate pictures of the keyword and translation referents. Second graders who generated their own linking images when given only the keywords and translation words recalled fewer items than both picture groups, and were not significantly different from control subjects. Sixth graders in the three imagery-link variations performed at comparable levels and better than control subjects. The results are in complete accord with previous speculations about the development of imagery strategies in children. 相似文献
93.
T R Barnes 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1978,25(1):71-79
The discriminative learning and transfer of compound and component problems were assessed in retarded subjects at two levels of intelligence. It was found that brighter subjects (MA = 10 years) found the component problems relatively easier than the compound problems to a greater extent than did the less developed subjects (MA = 5 years). The learning of compound solutions was shown to transfer positively to other compound problems and negatively to component problems. Component solutions were found to transfer positively to other component solutions and negatively to compound solutions. These dimensional transfer effects were interpreted to mean that both compound and component solutions were mediated and conceptual in nature. The developmental differences in relative usages of compound and component aspects of stimuli were viewed as the result of differences in direction of attention rather than of differences in complexity of processing. 相似文献
94.
Harry S Upshaw 《Journal of experimental social psychology》1978,14(4):327-339
Criteria for distinguishing between attitude and response scale effects in “after-only” studies of source-recipient discrepancy were developed and applied in two experiments. These criteria refer to congeneric scales (i.e., multiple measures of the the same property, which may differ in origin, unit, and reliability). Persuasive information may function as a scale anchor, in which case it is expected to affect only that response scale to which it directly pertains. Alternatively, it may convice recipients to change attitudinal positions, and produce effects on all congeneric scales. In one experiment, the self-proclaimed leniency-sternness of a presiding judge in a manslaughter case was varied along with the sentence he imposed and his trustworthiness. In the second study, only the sentence was manipulated, while both leniency ratings and sentencing were measured as dependent variables. Both analyses revealed support for the response scale interpretation, in that effects were observed only on those scales that were directly manipulated. 相似文献
95.
The purpose of this clinical experiment was to investigate the manipulability of a Factor II avoidance response (arm movement). The substantive hypothesis under test was that its suppression would be enhanced by repeated punishment sessions. During the first of nine therapy sessions response-contingent negative stimulation was not delivered. In the next five, after a base-rate period, “wrong” was made consequent upon the target response. In each of these sessions the suppression of arm movement was apparent. However, the magnitude of the suppression decreased from session to session. Evidently, the continued use of punishment did not enhance suppression. Because of this finding procedural changes were introduced in the next three therapy sessions. Verbal praise was added as a consequence of increasingly stringent omission criteria and three consecutive units of praise led to time-out from the reportedly fatiguing task of continuous speech. A marked reduction and then complete extinction resulted. Moreover, learning was evidenced since the frequency of arm movement lessened in the base-rate period of each session. 相似文献
96.
Martin R. Adams 《Journal of Fluency Disorders》1978,3(4):265-271
This article presents a further analysis of stuttering as a phonetic transition defect. Three types of phonetic transitions are identified. Two of these occur within words and represent sites where stutterings are readily detectable. The third transition involves movement from the end of one word to the beginning of the next. Spectrographic evidence is provided to show that stuttering also occurs at points of transition of this type. The same data are interpreted relative to their implications for stuttering theory, research, and therapy. 相似文献
97.
Norman R. Ellis 《Intelligence》1978,2(1):41-54
The hypothesis that mentally retarded persons have a short-term memory deficit whixh is due to inadequate rehearsal strategies is reexamined. A reevaluation of results of a number of experiments suggests that normal and retarded persons differ on short-term memory tasks, and the superiority of normals is present from the time of initial stimulus exposure. While some evidence for slope differences in retention curves is found in a few tasks, it is not strong support for memory differences. The hypothesis that “memory” differences are due to differential encoding as a result of more adequate rehearsal by the normal subjects is unacceptable. If this were the case, some differences in slope of retention curves could be expected unless rehearsal beyond the encoding stage was totally precluded. The review points to a need for an empirical reevaluation of the issue with a full recognition that deficits in particular processes may characterize some retarded persons and not others. 相似文献
98.
Herman H. Spitz 《Intelligence》1978,2(4):371-379
Mentally retarded individuals have relatively greater difficulty in dealing with strategic games and puzzles than with many other tasks. Consequently, it is of interest to find that strategic games have been played since ancient times, are played throughout the world, and that players who excel are frequently considered wise. Hidden within the more obvious differences between cultures, strategic games are an invariant expression of certain universal intellectual traits. 相似文献
99.
The attention demands of initiating and controlling discrete movements were examined as a function of their movement time (MT) and average movement velocity. Experiment 1 showed that the attention required to execute a movement decreased as MT decreased, although Experiment 2 through independently manipulating MT and movement velocity, revealed that movement velocity is the key determiner of attention demands rather than MT. The attention demands of preparing a high velocity movement are greater than during its execution with the reverse being the case for relatively slow velocity movements. The results are compatible with the view that it is the initiation of error corrections that are attention demanding (Keele 1973). 相似文献
100.