全文获取类型
收费全文 | 606篇 |
免费 | 530篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
1144篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 26篇 |
2018年 | 17篇 |
2017年 | 24篇 |
2016年 | 14篇 |
2015年 | 19篇 |
2014年 | 27篇 |
2013年 | 39篇 |
2012年 | 16篇 |
2011年 | 24篇 |
2010年 | 17篇 |
2009年 | 25篇 |
2008年 | 20篇 |
2007年 | 28篇 |
2006年 | 34篇 |
2005年 | 31篇 |
2004年 | 28篇 |
2003年 | 18篇 |
2002年 | 19篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 22篇 |
1996年 | 41篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 26篇 |
1993年 | 29篇 |
1992年 | 37篇 |
1991年 | 21篇 |
1990年 | 23篇 |
1989年 | 29篇 |
1988年 | 23篇 |
1987年 | 31篇 |
1986年 | 19篇 |
1985年 | 26篇 |
1984年 | 24篇 |
1983年 | 21篇 |
1982年 | 28篇 |
1981年 | 25篇 |
1980年 | 25篇 |
1979年 | 26篇 |
1978年 | 30篇 |
1977年 | 31篇 |
1976年 | 33篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
排序方式: 共有1144条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
111.
Pigeons were exposed to a stimulus fading procedure in which control of responding was transferred from red and black stimuli to lines of different angular orientation. After superimposing one line on the red stimulus and the other line on the black stimulus, the intensity of the lines was gradually increased and that of the red stimulus was gradually reduced. Probes consisting of red and line stimuli presented separately were used during the course of fading to assess control exerted by each element of the compound. As the lines were faded in, they did not acquire control of responding. As red was faded out, control of responding was acquired first by the lower intensity red stimuli in combination with the line stimulus, and finally by the angular orientation of the lines. Probes also determined the point at which the line stimuli, presented alone, would maintain a high degree of stimulus control. The results demonstrated that new stimuli in fading acquire dimensional control of responding in two sequential stages. Acquisition of stimulus control in fading was explained in terms of attenuation of stimulus blocking. 相似文献
112.
Parker BK Serdikoff SL Kaminski BJ Critchfield TS 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1991,55(3):275-286
Two experiments were conducted using an autoshaping procedure with pigeons to examine whether dimensional stimulus control by a Pavlovian facilitator parallels the control established following operant discrimination training. Facilitation training consisted of the presentation of a black vertical line on a white background as the B stimulus in a feature-positive discrimination in which the A stimulus (white keylight) was followed by grain presentation only if preceded by B. In this way, B facilitates or sets the occasion for pecking at A. Subsequent testing for generalization along the line-orientation dimension produced decremental gradients when the facilitation paradigm incorporated an explicit feature-negative stimulus (B−). These results parallel the decremental control obtained following operant discrimination training and suggest that Pavlovian facilitators and instrumental discriminative stimuli are functionally equivalent. 相似文献
113.
In a symbolic matching-to-sample task, 6 pigeons obtained food by pecking a red side key when the brighter of two white lights had been presented on the center key and by pecking a green side key when the dimmer of two white lights had been presented on the center key. Across Part 1 and Parts 6 to 10, the delay between sample-stimulus presentation and the availability of the choice keys was varied between 0 s and 25 s. Across Parts 1 to 5, the delay between the emission of a correct choice and the delivery of a reinforcer was varied between 0 s and 30 s. Although increasing both types of delay decreased stimulus discriminability, lengthening the stimulus-choice delay produced a greater decrement in choice accuracy than did lengthening the choice-reinforcer delay. Additionally, the relative reinforcer rate for correct choice was varied across both types of delay. The sensitivity of behavior to the distribution of reinforcers decreased as discriminability decreased under both procedures. These data are consistent with the view, based on the generalized matching law, that sample stimuli and reinforcers interact in their control over remembering. 相似文献
114.
Analysis of the elements of attention: A neuropsychological approach 总被引:15,自引:0,他引:15
Allan F. Mirsky Bruno J. Anthony Connie C. Duncan Mary Beth Ahearn Sheppard G. Kellam 《Neuropsychology review》1991,2(2):109-145
A model for conceptualizing the components or elements of attention is presented. The model substitutes for the diffuse and global concept of attention a group of four processes and links them to a putative system of cerebral structures. Data in support of the model are presented; they are derived from neuropsychological test scores obtained from two samples, the first consisting of 203 adult neuropsychiatric patients and normal control subjects, and the second, an epidemiologically-based sample of 435 elementary school children. Principal components analyses of test scores from these two populations yielded similar results: a set of independent elements of attention that are assayed by different tests. This work presents a heuristic for clinical research in which the measurement of attention is essential. 相似文献
115.
邱鸿钟 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2007,28(4):14-16
运用科学哲学、后现代主义哲学、后殖民批判关于科学认识论、方法论问题的观点,讨论了中医药存在的合理性和价值问题,认为中医是对生活世界和主体性的回归,是另一种科学或文化范式,中西医都是各民族原创的具有同等地位的元叙说方式和文本。 相似文献
116.
Four pigeons were exposed to independent concurrent variable-interval 20-second variable-interval 60-second schedules of reinforcement. A transparent partition was inserted midway between the two response keys. The length of the partition was systematically manipulated. Increasing partition length produced a decrease in changeover rate in Experiment 1. Over-matching was observed with a partition length of 20 centimeters. In Experiment 2 a four-second limited hold was added to the schedules. Increasing partition length produced a decrease in changeover rate that exceeded the decrease observed in Experiment 1. This manipulation produced nearly exclusive choice of the variable-interval 20-second component. The present results, together with results obtained in related research, suggest that deviation from matching is a function of procedural variables that determine the consequences of a changeover response. 相似文献
117.
讨论医院重点专科发展过程的阶段划分及特征,对于我们在重点专科建设中正确地把握方向、恰当地评估成效和有力地予以推进都有十分重大和现实的意义。本文把医院重点专科发展过程分为科室建设、学科建设、科学建设阶段,并从功能目标、效率效益、人才队伍、场地设施、技术设备、技术特色、教学培训、科学研究、文化氛围、能级水平十个方面对各阶段的特征进行归纳描述。三个阶段各具不同内涵特征,又存在相互内在关联,具有方向性、连续性、可加速性、交叉性和包涵性等规律。 相似文献
118.
119.
Ping Lin Ling Li Yini Wang Zhenjuan Zhao Guojie Liu Wei Chen 《Psychology, health & medicine》2018,23(2):216-223
Personality traits are associated with major adverse coronary events (MACE) in patients with coronary artery disease (CAD). However, the link between personality traits and intravascular morphology in CAD patients is poorly understood. This study investigated the relationship between personality traits, specifically Type A behavior pattern and Type D personality, and plaque vulnerability. After adjustment for demographic and clinical factors, multivariable regression analysis showed no association between Type A and optical coherence tomography indices. However, Type D personality was independently associated with lipid plaque, thin cap fibroatheroma (TCFA), and fibrous cap thickness. More specifically, negative affectivity of Type D was related to lipid plaque, TCFA and fibrous cap thickness, and social inhibition was associated with plaque rupture. Our results show that Type D personality was associated with plaque vulnerability, independent of clinical factors. Measurement of negative affectivity and social inhibition will increase our understanding of the progressive phase of the plaque vulnerability, which can contribute to the early identification of high risk patients and reduce the incidence of MACE. 相似文献
120.