全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1011篇 |
免费 | 104篇 |
国内免费 | 39篇 |
专业分类
1154篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 3篇 |
2024年 | 23篇 |
2023年 | 25篇 |
2022年 | 20篇 |
2021年 | 36篇 |
2020年 | 59篇 |
2019年 | 62篇 |
2018年 | 57篇 |
2017年 | 62篇 |
2016年 | 57篇 |
2015年 | 38篇 |
2014年 | 43篇 |
2013年 | 137篇 |
2012年 | 43篇 |
2011年 | 37篇 |
2010年 | 35篇 |
2009年 | 31篇 |
2008年 | 43篇 |
2007年 | 34篇 |
2006年 | 45篇 |
2005年 | 40篇 |
2004年 | 28篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 47篇 |
2001年 | 30篇 |
2000年 | 15篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有1154条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
891.
Dimensions of martial conflict, children's emotional security regarding interparental conflict, and parenting style were examined
as mediators between parental dysphoria and child adjustment. A community sample of 262 children, ages 8–16, participated
with their parents. Behavioral observations were made of parents’ interactions during marital conflict resolution tasks, which
children later observed to assess their emotional security. Questionnaires assessed parents’ dysphoria, parenting, and children's
adjustment. Structural equation modeling indicated that parental dysphoria was linked with child adjustment through specific
and distinct mediating family processes, including marital conflict and parenting. Children's emotional security in the context
of particular marital conflict styles also mediated relations between parental dysphoria and child adjustment problems, with
similar pathways found for mothers and fathers. These pathways remained significant even after significant parenting contributions
were considered. 相似文献
892.
Lisa Sheeber Hyman Hops Anthony Alpert Betsy Davis Judy Andrews 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1997,25(4):333-344
The relations between family support, family conflict, and adolescent depressive symptomatology were examined longitudinally in a sample of 231 female and 189 male adolescents and their mothers. Structural equation models revealed that less supportive and more con-flictual family environments were associated with greater depressive symptomatology both concurrently and prospectively over a 1-year period. Conversely, adolescent depressive symptomatology did not predict deterioration in family relationships. Depressive symptomatology and, to a greater extent, family characteristics showed high levels of stability over the 1-year period. Counter to our expectations, the relations between family variables and depressive symptomatology were similar for boys and girls. The results suggest that the quality of family interactions is relevant for understanding the development of depressive symptoms in adolescents. 相似文献
893.
H.TristramEngelhardt 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2002,23(10):1-5
世界上存在着道德多元性,它表现为生命伦理学的实际道德分布的特征,它是使用人类胚胎作为干细胞来源的争论的根本原因。在世俗文化中,干细胞的使用甚至也会出现疑问。只以从传统的基督教道德直觉和期望的残留影响角度去理解这种现象。关于干细胞的争论反映了更深层次的文化错位。我们正生活在文化战争频发的年代。有必要对罗尔斯学派的关于公共讨论及理性的道德多元化给予重新的关注和评价。 相似文献
894.
《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2013,66(4):779-801
Three experiments investigated transfer of list-wide proportion congruent (LWPC) effects from a set of congruent and incongruent items with different frequency (inducer task) to a set of congruent and incongruent items with equal frequency (diagnostic task). Experiments 1 and 2 mixed items from horizontal and vertical Simon tasks. Tasks always involved different stimuli that varied on the same dimension (colour) in Experiment 1 and on different dimensions (colour, shape) in Experiment 2. Experiment 3 mixed trials from a manual Simon task with trials from a vocal Stroop task, with colour being the relevant stimulus in both tasks. There were two major results. First, we observed transfer of LWPC effects in Experiments 1 and 3, when tasks shared the relevant dimension, but not in Experiment 2. Second, sequential modulations of congruency effects transferred in Experiment 1 only. Hence, the different transfer patterns suggest that LWPC effects and sequential modulations arise from different mechanisms. Moreover, the observation of transfer supports an account of LWPC effects in terms of list-wide cognitive control, while being at odds with accounts in terms of stimulus–response (contingency) learning and item-specific control. 相似文献
895.
Jeffrey A. Kurland 《Zygon》1999,34(1):67-92
Ever since its inception, Charles Darwin's theory of evolution by natural selection has challenged assumptions about the nature of humankind and human institutions. It did not escape the notice of Darwin, sympathetic allies, or hostile contemporaries that his theory had profound implications for ethics and theology. In this paper I review some current sociobiological hypotheses about the mind that are based on the theory that the human mind is primarily a social tool. Many researchers now believe that both complex human within-group cooperation and between-group competition are the anvils that may have shaped the modules of the mind. Given this evolutionary theory of the mind, the Darwinian challenge to theism, ethics, and faith is now being relaunched with a vengeance. However, I suggest that modern physics, evolutionary biology, and cognitive science all seem to fit nicely into the atheistic and phenomenological niche defined by Buddhism. 相似文献
896.
Researchers have begun to develop models that explain the processes by which interparental conflict impacts children's adjustment. The present study tested a model based on emotional security theory. The longitudinal relations among interparental conflict, boys' reactions to conflict, and internalizing and externalizing problems were examined in a sample of 129 mother–son dyads from low-income, 2-parent families from the time sons were age 2 to 5. Results indicated that children exposed to interparental conflict were more likely to have concurrent and later behavior problems and that patterns of interparental conflict across time made unique contributions in predicting later problems. Children's emotional reactivity in response to conflict had no direct relation to interparental conflict and only modest relations to behavior problems. However, interparental conflict and reactivity factors interacted to predict behavior problems at ages 3 1/2 and 5. Thus, some support was demonstrated for emotional reactivity as a moderator in the development of young children's behavior problems. 相似文献
897.
Daniel Bar-Tal 《Political psychology》2000,21(2):351-365
Intractable intergroup conflicts require the formation of a conflictive ethos that enables a society to adapt to the conflict situation, survive the stressful period, and struggle successfully with the adversary. The formal termination of such a conflict begins with the elimination of the perceived incompatibility between the opposing parties through negotiation by their representatives—that is, a conflict resolution process. But this is only part of the long-term reconciliation process, which requires the formation of peaceful relations based on mutual trust and acceptance, cooperation, and consideration of mutual needs. The psychological aspect of reconciliation requires a change in the conflictive ethos, especially with respect to societal beliefs about group goals, about the adversary group, about the ingroup, about intergroup relations, and about the nature of peace. In essence, psychological reconciliation requires the formation of an ethos of peace, but this is extremely difficult in cases of intractable conflict. Political psychologists can and should work to improve the state of knowledge about reconciliation,which until now has received much less attention than conflict resolution. 相似文献
898.
Social agonism and dominance relations were assessed in two preschool groups. An average of 30 hours of observational data was obtained for each group during free-play over a six-week period. A Social Agonism Inventory describing specific initiation and response behaviors during episodes of social conflict was used to code agonistic activity. Dyadic interactions were examined to determine whether the resolution of agonistic interactions conformed to a linear model of social dominance. Although there were group differences in the relative frequency of different forms of conflict, systematic comparison of agonistic wins and losses re vealed similarly rigid and linear dominance structures at both preschools. The relation of aggression and dominance was empirically examined by comparing the initiation and receipt of agonism by high vs low dominance status children. Higher dominance status children engaged in more agonistic interactions, but a majority of these interactions were directed toward other high-ranking group members. 相似文献
899.
900.
医务人员在医疗活动中的利益边界与道德责任分析 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
郑大喜 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2008,29(7)
当今是一个以利益为轴心的时代,利益伦理是医学伦理学不可忽视的课题。欲望是无穷的,利益是有限的,任何利益都有其边界和限度,我们必须设置利益的道德界限。医患双方未能遵守利益限度和利益边界原则,使矛盾转化为冲突。利益冲突不仅影响医生的专业判断,而且影响了其医疗决定。医生既是医疗职业者,也是社会的一员,社会公众利益最大化是医疗服务的基本目标,为病人最大利益着想是医护专业最根本的道德规范与责任。 相似文献