全文获取类型
收费全文 | 186篇 |
免费 | 51篇 |
国内免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
247篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 8篇 |
2017年 | 5篇 |
2016年 | 6篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 14篇 |
2012年 | 7篇 |
2011年 | 11篇 |
2010年 | 1篇 |
2009年 | 6篇 |
2008年 | 9篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 8篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 12篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有247条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
92.
93.
The spontaneous tendency to join the largest social group was used to investigate quantity discrimination in fish. Fish discriminated between shoals that differed by one element when the paired numbers were 1vs2, 2vs3 and 3vs4, but not when 4vs5 or larger. Using large numerosities (>4), the ability to discriminate between two numbers improved as the numerical distance between them increased and a significant discrimination was found only with ratios of 1:2 or smaller (4vs8, 8vs16 and 4vs10). Experiments to control for non-numerical variables evidenced the role played by the total area of stimuli with both large and small numerosities; the total quantity of movement of the fish within a shoal appeared also important but only when large numerosities were involved. Even though the pattern of discrimination exhibited by female mosquitofish is not fully consistent with any of the existing models of quantity representation, our results seem to suggest two distinct mechanisms in fish, one used to compare small numbers of objects and one used when larger numerosities are involved. 相似文献
94.
Behavior maintained with 2-component concurrent variable interval schedules of reinforcement (CONC VIVI) is described well by the matching law. Deviations from matching behavior have been handled by adding free parameters to the matching law equation. With CONC VIVI schedules there are infinitely many solutions to the matching law equation at each value of the procedural parameters. However, at each value of the procedural parameters, only one combination of durations of intervals spent in each VI component (dwell times) yields the combined maximum reinforcement rate. The equations that yield the optimal dwell times solution for CONC VIVI schedules are mathematically incompatible with the matching law. Optimal performance and matching coincide only when the parameter values of the two VI components are equal. It seems reasonable to use optimal behavior to assess performance in these schedules. Researchers have not compared optimal and empirical performances in CONC VIVI possibly because the equations for optimal dwell times (ODT) can be solved only numerically. We present a table of ODT for a wide range of VIs and changeover delays. We also derive a function m that can be used to compare matching data and the matching behavior predictions of optimization. We prove that 0.5<m<1.003502, and we describe some of the more nteresting properties of the function. 相似文献
95.
Modal Foundations for Predicate Logic 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
96.
本研究通过蒙特卡洛模拟考查了采用全息极大似然估计进行缺失数据建模时辅助变量的作用。具体考查了辅助变量与研究变量的共缺机制、共缺率、相关程度、辅助变量数目与样本量等因素对参数估计结果精确性的影响。结果表明,当辅助与研究变量共缺时:(1)对于完全随机缺失的辅助变量,结果更容易出现偏差;(2)对于MAR-MAR组合机制,纳入单个辅助变量是有益的;对于MAR-MCAR或MAR-MNAR组合机制,纳入多于一个辅助变量的效果更好;(3)纳入与研究变量低相关的辅助变量对结果也是有益的。 相似文献
97.
Concurrent variable ratio-variable interval (CONC VRVI) schedules of reinforcement, and the time-based analog of the same schedule (CONC VT*VT), have been used to determine if the matching law accounts for the distribution of choices between the behavior alternatives more accurately than the assumption that subjects distribute time between the alternatives to maximize total reinforcement rate. The results of those experiments leave room for interpretation. One problem is the lack of understanding of the theoretical outcomes associated with maximization in these schedules. A precise understanding of the characteristics of optimal behavior (OB) could help identify experimental evidence of OB. Here we derive equations that describe the optimal times the subject should spend on each alternative of the schedule. We provide a table of the optimal times for a wide range of parameter values of the schedule that experimenters can use to compare easily experimental results to the results expected if subjects behave optimally. We also derive a function m that relates matching and optimal performance and we prove interesting characteristics of the function. Finally, we describe features of OB with CONC VT*VT and with concurrent variable time schedules that can be used to identify evidence of OB. 相似文献
98.
Ward A 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》2007,28(6):465-485
The goal of research in social epidemiology is not simply conceptual clarification or theoretical understanding, but more
importantly it is to contribute to, and enhance the health of populations (and so, too, the people who constitute those populations).
Undoubtedly, understanding how various individual risk factors such as smoking and obesity affect the health of people does
contribute to this goal. However, what is distinctive of much on-going work in social epidemiology is the view that analyses
making use of individual-level variables is not enough. In the spirit of Durkheim and Weber, S. Leonard Syme makes this point
by writing that just “as bad water and food may be harmful to our health, unhealthful forces in our society may be detrimental
to our capacity to make choices and to form opinions” conducive to health and well-being. Advocates of upstream (distal) causes
of adverse health outcomes propose to identify the most important of these “unhealthful forces” as the fundamental causes
of adverse health outcomes. However, without a clear, theoretically precise and well-grounded understanding of the characteristics
of fundamental causes, there is little hope in applying the statistical tools of the health sciences to hypotheses about fundamental
causes, their outcomes, and policies intended to enhance the health of populations. This paper begins the process of characterizing
the social epidemiological concept of fundamental cause in a theoretically respectable and robust way.
相似文献
Andrew WardEmail: |
99.
The construct of psychophysiological reactivity: Statistical and psychometric issues 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The purpose of this paper is to review major statistical and psychometric issues impacting the study of psychophysiological reactivity and discuss their implications for applied developmental researchers. We first cover traditional approaches such as the observed difference score (DS) and the observed residual score (RS), including a review of classic and recent research on their reliability and validity from two related bodies of work: the measurement of change and the Law of Initial Values. Second, we review several types of latent variable modeling in this context: latent difference score (LDS) models, latent residual score (LRS) models, latent state-trait (LST) models, and latent growth curve (LGC) models. Finally, we provide broad guidelines for applied researchers broken down by key stages of a psychophysiological project: study planning, data analysis, and reporting of results. Our recommendations highlight the need for (1) increased attention to the ubiquitous nature of measurement error in observed variables and the importance of employing latent variable models when possible, and (2) increased specification of theories relating to the construct of reactivity, especially in regards to the distinction between baseline arousal and change over time in broader systems of variables. 相似文献
100.
采用问卷法以743名中小学在职教师为被试研究了教学效能感在职业压力影响职业倦怠过程中的调节作用。结果发现:①教师的工作特征、学生等压力源能有效预测教师职业倦怠。随着从业年限的增加,影响职业倦怠的主要压力源从工作特征过渡到社会特征。②教学效能的调节作用因教师不同的人口学分布而异,其中一般教学效能和个体教学效能所表现出来的调节效应也不同。③个体教学效能表现出线性调节作用,一般教学效能表现出曲线调节作用,两种调节作用的性质取决于不同的压力源。 相似文献