全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2963篇 |
免费 | 363篇 |
国内免费 | 82篇 |
专业分类
3408篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 28篇 |
2023年 | 51篇 |
2022年 | 70篇 |
2021年 | 79篇 |
2020年 | 175篇 |
2019年 | 185篇 |
2018年 | 167篇 |
2017年 | 185篇 |
2016年 | 152篇 |
2015年 | 137篇 |
2014年 | 145篇 |
2013年 | 580篇 |
2012年 | 85篇 |
2011年 | 145篇 |
2010年 | 83篇 |
2009年 | 111篇 |
2008年 | 109篇 |
2007年 | 111篇 |
2006年 | 81篇 |
2005年 | 74篇 |
2004年 | 74篇 |
2003年 | 54篇 |
2002年 | 45篇 |
2001年 | 39篇 |
2000年 | 37篇 |
1999年 | 22篇 |
1998年 | 22篇 |
1997年 | 20篇 |
1996年 | 21篇 |
1995年 | 22篇 |
1994年 | 22篇 |
1993年 | 22篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 14篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 17篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 29篇 |
1976年 | 17篇 |
1975年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有3408条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
131.
The present investigation examined the singular and interactive effects of anxiety sensitivity and perceived control over anxiety-related events in the prediction of panic symptoms using a CO(2)-enriched air biological challenge. Two hundred and twenty-nine adult participants (M(age)=21.02, SD=7.55, 124 females) were recruited from the greater Burlington, Vermont community. Results indicated that pre-challenge anxiety sensitivity, but not perceived control over anxiety-related events, significantly predicted post-challenge panic attack symptoms, anxiety focused on bodily sensations, and, interest in returning for another challenge (behavioral avoidance). In regard to physiological findings, anxiety sensitivity was significantly related to skin conductance level whereas perceived control over anxiety-related events was related to respiration rate. Neither anxiety sensitivity nor perceived control over anxiety-related events was related to heart rate. There also were no interactive effects between anxiety sensitivity and perceived control over anxiety-related events for any of the studied dependent variables. Results are discussed in relation to multi-risk factor models of cognitive vulnerability for panic psychopathology. 相似文献
132.
Jean-Philippe Vauthier 《Médecine & Droit》2021,2021(166):11-16
Although of old and common use, the use of isolation and restraint in psychiatry was not regulated until 2016 with the creation of article L. 3222-5-1 of the Public Health Code. However, the Court of Cassation considered that, as a form of care, it was beyond the office of the liberty and custody judge to controle its implementation. Questioned by a plea of unconstitutionality submitted by the Court of Cassation, the Constitutional Council considered, with its decision of 19 June 2020, that these measures constituted a deprivation of liberty requiring judicial review at short notice. The Sages thus declare the provisions of article L. 3222-5-1 unconstitutional, with delayed effect as of December 31, 2020, requiring legislator to rethink and organise the supervision and control of decisions of restraint and isolation. 相似文献
133.
Three students with moderate disabilities were taught to read and match-to-sample sight words comprising stimulus sets based upon the four food groups. We taught students conditional discriminations within four four-member sets using a single-sample/four-comparison procedure. Students were taught A-B, B-C, and C-D conditional discriminations for each of the four potential stimulus classes. Subsequent probes tested for relations based upon symmetry and one-node and two-node transitivity. The performances for all students indicated that symmetric relations emerged before one-node transitive relations, and that one-node transitive relations emerged before two-node transitive relations. These results are consistent with a pattern of responding, referred to as a "nodality effect," in which relations with fewer nodes are demonstrated prior to the demonstration of relations with a greater number of nodes. These results extend this area of research to sight-word reading for students with moderate disabilities. 相似文献
134.
Intra-individual reaction time variability (IIV) in neuropsychological task performance reflects short term fluctuations in performance. Increased IIV has been reported in patients with schizophrenia and could be related to a deficient neural timing mechanism, but the role of IIV in adult patients with other psychiatric disorders has not been established. Therefore, we compared IIV measures obtained in a Go/Nogo task from patients with schizophrenia, major depression and borderline personality disorder. IIV was increased for patients with schizophrenia. When correcting for differences in mean reaction time, depressive and borderline patients also showed increased IIV. Importantly, all groups showed a strong association between IIV and accuracy of task performance. This suggests that increased IIV might be a sensitive marker for the efficiency of top-down attentional control in all diagnostic groups. Aside from these similarities, the complete results including measures of IIV, mean reaction time and accuracy show differential patterns for patients with schizophrenia compared to those with borderline personality disorder or depression. These results are discussed with respect to common versus disorder-specific neural mechanisms underlying increased IIV. 相似文献
135.
焦虑个体“冷”、“热”执行功能Stroop效应量比较 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
该文采用Stroop实验范式对筛选出的49名高焦虑个体和48名低焦虑个体的“冷”、“热”执行功能进行测量,以探讨不同焦虑程度个体“冷”、“热”执行功能间的差异。结果表明,在“冷”执行功能方面,高焦虑个体与低焦虑个体Stroop效应量之间没有显著差异;在“热”执行功能方面,两组被试情绪Stroop的效应量无差异;两类词;12的Stroop效应量差异显著;词汇类型×被试类型的交互作用非常显著;进一步的简单效应分析表明,两组被试的情绪Stroop效应量在消极词汇水平差异显著,两类词汇Stroop效应量在低焦虑组差异显著。说明高、低焦虑个体在“冷”执行功能上没有显著差异,高焦虑个体的“冷”执行功能正常,而“热”执行功能下降,表现为高焦虑个体对两极情绪的趋避关系失调,即对消极情绪具有偏向性,对积极情绪具有回避性。 相似文献
136.
基于"大学生互联网使用动机量表"和"大学生病理性互联网使用行为量表"的测量结果,采用匹配法筛选出21名基于人际情感性动机的病理性网络用户和22名非人际情感性动机的健康性网络用户,对其实施延迟折扣任务(DDT)、stop-signal任务和Stroop色词命名任务,探讨了基于人际情感性动机的病理性网络用户的冲动控制特性。结果发现,相对于健康性网络用户,病理性网络用户对延迟强化物的折扣速度更快,但后者的反应抑制失败率、反应抑制潜伏期(SSRT)和冲突抑制功能均与前者无显著差异。这些结果表明,病理性网络用户的冲动控制障碍主要表现为以对即时强化的超敏感性和强化物长远价值的非敏感性为特征的"短视"倾向,而非行为抑制功能的缺失。 相似文献
137.
A group of young children (mean age: 2.5 yr) were instructed to follow different requests by a teacher in a day-care setting. Experiment I verified that mean group instruction following was low (10%) despite the opportunity for “observational learning”, i.e., the group of 12 children could watch a nonreinforced adult comply with the teacher's request. In Experiment II, when positive consequences were provided contingent on the adult model's behavior, mean group instruction following was relatively unaffected (14%). When direct reinforcement was given to four peer models, each for several sessions, the individual performances of three of the four peer models was elevated (from 50% to 80%); however, the mean performance of the remaining nonreinforced children (N = 7) was only moderately affected (21%). When reinforcement contingencies were again changed, so that each group member was provided direct, but intermittent reinforcement, mean group performance increased substantially to levels of over 70%. Once instruction following was high, presentation of reinforcement only to one peer model sufficed to maintain performance whereas earlier, this same vicarious reinforcement procedure had failed to establish group compliance. The maintenance of instruction-following behavior when reinforcement was applied solely to one child was interpreted mainly in terms of a high resistance to extinction following a history of intermittent reinforcement rather than a “vicarious”- or “self”-reinforcement mechanism. Finally, removal and re-introduction of group intermittent reinforcement, respectively, lowered performance (to levels of 40%) and elevated (to levels of 65%) the group's performance. 相似文献
138.
Richard S. Surwit Joanne L. Hager Theodore Feldman 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1977,10(4):625-631
Forty normal male volunteers were randomly assigned to one of four experimental conditions and instructed to raise and lower their systolic blood pressure. Subjects received either beat-to-beat feedback contingent on pressure changes, noncontingent beat-to-beat feedback, noncontingent feedback presented randomly with respect to the occurrence of each heart beat, or instructions alone. The order of increase and decrease trial blocks was counterbalanced across groups. Subjects receiving contingent feedback were monetarily rewarded for appropriate pressure changes. Subjects receiving noncontingent feedback received rewards and feedback equal to the mean received by the contingent group. Subjects in the instructions-only condition were also paid this bonus but were informed of their earnings only at the conclusion of the experiment. Results indicated that in the presence of instructions, feedback, whether contingent or noncontingent, added little to subjects' ability to control pressure during a single session. Theoretical and clinical implications are discussed. 相似文献
139.
Michael T. Christensen 《Aggressive behavior》1977,3(1):17-31
Genetically sterile male Norway rats, Rattus norvegicus, were tested in the laboratory to determine both 1) behavioral characteristics, and 2) the ability of sterile males to compete aggressively and sexually with wild Norway rat males. Sterile males were larger in weight, more frequently dominant, won as many fighting encounters, were as aggressive as wild males, and mounted females more frequently. Behavioral activities were similar for both strains when compared under laboratory conditions with no apparent abnormal behavior exhibited by the sterile males. Use of sterile males in biological control programs is discussed. 相似文献
140.
通过突发公共卫生事件来审视目前我国医院感染管理工作中存在的一系列问题并提出了相应对策. 相似文献