全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4853篇 |
免费 | 514篇 |
国内免费 | 158篇 |
专业分类
5525篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 8篇 |
2024年 | 70篇 |
2023年 | 81篇 |
2022年 | 79篇 |
2021年 | 141篇 |
2020年 | 225篇 |
2019年 | 290篇 |
2018年 | 303篇 |
2017年 | 365篇 |
2016年 | 333篇 |
2015年 | 151篇 |
2014年 | 197篇 |
2013年 | 1146篇 |
2012年 | 125篇 |
2011年 | 219篇 |
2010年 | 135篇 |
2009年 | 186篇 |
2008年 | 198篇 |
2007年 | 174篇 |
2006年 | 143篇 |
2005年 | 152篇 |
2004年 | 135篇 |
2003年 | 120篇 |
2002年 | 93篇 |
2001年 | 75篇 |
2000年 | 41篇 |
1999年 | 59篇 |
1998年 | 56篇 |
1997年 | 22篇 |
1996年 | 33篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 26篇 |
1993年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 17篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有5525条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
72.
Michael Schwartze Rachel M. Brown Emmanuel Biau Sonja A. Kotz 《International journal of psychology》2020,55(3):342-346
The informative value of time and temporal structure often remains neglected in cognitive assessments. However, next to information about stimulus identity we can exploit temporal ordering principles, such as regularity, periodicity, or grouping to generate predictions about the timing of future events. Such predictions may improve cognitive performance by optimising adaptation to dynamic stimuli. Here, we investigated the influence of temporal structure on verbal working memory by assessing immediate recall performance for aurally presented digit sequences (forward digit span) as a function of standard (1000 ms stimulus-onset-asynchronies, SOAs), short (700 ms), long (1300 ms) and mixed (700–1300 ms) stimulus timing during the presentation phase. Participant's digit spans were lower for short and mixed SOA presentation relative to standard SOAs. This confirms an impact of temporal structure on the classic “magical number seven,” suggesting that working memory performance can in part be regulated through the systematic application of temporal ordering principles. 相似文献
73.
Amanda Dowd 《The Journal of analytical psychology》2020,65(2):300-324
Originally presented at the Journal’s one day conference entitled ‘Displacement: Contemporary Traumatic Experience’ held in London in November 2019, this paper expands on the author’s theory of the implicit psychological organizing gestalt, an associated pattern of psychic functions which operate in an integrated way to simultaneously structure and organize our experience of self-cohesion and self-continuity. The gestalt, which implicitly links the formation of psychic skin, body image, cultural skin and both personal and cultural identity with place, functions as an emergent non-conscious permanent presence or background ‘constant’. It develops over time and emerges out of embodied emotional experiencing with the total environment – both human and non-human. The author argues that it is the rupture of this gestalt and the disorganizing consequences of its loss which underlies the experience of displacement trauma. If disruptions in the formation of the gestalt and/or its later rupture remain unrecognized and unrepresented then the absence creates a void which can be intergenerationally transmitted. Case material is presented which describes this and which highlights the ways in which the gestalt can contribute to our understanding of collective displacement anxiety, cultural trauma and cultural complexes. 相似文献
74.
Carmen E. Westerberg Nathan Wofford Safia Menssor Bryant P. Reininger Rebecca G. Deason 《Journal of Cognitive Psychology》2020,32(7):587-597
ABSTRACT Faces belonging to the same category as the perceiver are better recognised than faces from different categories when tested immediately. After a delay, the same-category benefit persists and response bias becomes more liberal for other-category but not same-category faces. These effects are typically attributed to better encoding of same-category than other-category faces. To determine whether bias effects after a delay persist when immediate accuracy differences are minimised, Hispanic and Caucasian participants were given more study time for other-category than same-category faces. Memory was tested immediately and after a two-day delay. Discrimination accuracy was higher for other-category than for same-category faces during both test sessions. Bias was equivalent immediately, but after the delay, bias was more conservative for same-category than for other-category faces, especially in Caucasian participants. These results suggest that post-encoding processes differentially influence face memories from different categories, which may have implications for evaluating face memory in everyday situations. 相似文献
75.
Emma Barkus 《PsyCh Journal》2020,9(2):258-279
Working memory training is widely used transdiagnostically to improve cognition. However, more recently, studies using working memory training packages have targeted emotion‐regulation outcomes to determine whether far transfer effects can be achieved. A narrative review is conducted of studies that have used standardized computerized working memory training packages across healthy volunteers, affect, anxiety, post‐traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and eating disordered populations with emotion‐regulation outcomes. Working memory training has been used in children, adolescents, and adults to improve emotion regulation. Many studies have reported gains in mood as well as emotion‐regulation strategies following working memory training, regardless of clinical indication and whether near transfer gains were achieved in cognitive domains. Significant emotion‐regulation outcomes include: state and trait anxiety, rumination, brooding, positive appraisal, decreasing maladaptive emotion‐regulation strategies, and decreasing intrusive thoughts. It is speculated that these far transfer outcomes from working memory training are possible due to the cognitive and neural overlap between cognitive and affective working memory, and emotion regulation. Working memory training could improve cognitive efficiency, which, in turn, increases the availability of cognitive resources during times when emotion regulation is taxed. Future studies need to consider the role of participant expectancy in predicting outcome measure performance, and including subjective and objective outcomes is paramount to study design. Furthermore, sample sizes require additional attention, given that the current review highlights that individual differences in non‐clinical and clinical populations influence the outcomes from working memory training. Working memory training offers a possibility for improving emotion regulation transdiagnostically. 相似文献
76.
Can neurosciences explain art? No, but it can help us to understand why some images are more memorable and, thus, more successful than others. This article aims to identify certain factors that may influence the artistic success of photographic images. These factors are discussed within the contexts of basic neuropsychological concepts, visual perception, and visual memory. A new computational and neuroscientifically based model, the predictive coding theory, provides a powerful framework for integrating social and individual factors that influence aesthetic experience and activity. A case study of Dorothea Lange's iconic photograph Migrant Mother demonstrates the importance of identifiable factors that influence and determine a photograph's potential success. We are convinced that a future systemic approach will enable the complementary integration of neuroscientific, perceptual, cognitive, emotional, and sociopsychological insights through the framework of predictive coding theory with socioscientific, art‐theoretical, and art‐historical as well as neuro‐ and behavioral‐economical models. 相似文献
77.
The hypothesis that borderline personality disorder (BPD) is related to overgeneral memories was tested in a mixed sample of 39 patients. A memory test with emotional cue words and the instruction to produce specific autobiographical memories was used. Specificity was judged by an independent rater. Regression analyses indicated that age and major depressive disorder were related to the production of less specific memories, whereas educational level and presence of personality disorder were positivily related to number of specific memories. Borderline personality disorder, anxiety disorders and childhood traumas were not related to number of specific memories. 相似文献
78.
Crown CL Feldstein S Jasnow MD Beebe B Jaffe J 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》2002,31(1):1-23
The purpose of this study was to examine the hypothesis that 6-week-old infants are capable of coordinated interpersonal timing within social interactions. Coordinated interpersonal timing refers to changes in the timing of one individual's behavior as a function of the timing of another individual's behavior. Each of 45, first-born 6-week-old infants interacted with his or her mother and a stranger for a total of 14 minutes. The interactions were videotaped and coded for the gaze behavior of the infants and the vocal behavior of the mothers and strangers. Time-series regression analyses were used to assess the extent to which the timing of each of the infants' gazes was coordinated with the timing of the adults' vocal behavior. The results revealed that (a) coordinated timing occurs between infants and their mothers and between infants and strangers as early as when infants are 6 weeks old, and (b) strangers coordinated the timing of their pauses with the infants to a greater extent than did mothers. The findings are discussed in terms of the role of temporal sensitivity in social interaction. 相似文献
79.
An adult California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) with extensive experience in performing discrimination learning tasks was tested to evaluate her long-term memory for two
previously learned concepts. An associative concept, that of equivalence classification, was retested after a retention interval
of approximately 1 year. The sea lion had originally shown emergent equivalence classification with nonsimilarity-based classes
of stimuli in a simple discrimination repeated-reversal procedure as well as in a matching-to-sample procedure. The 1-year
memory test revealed no decrement in classification performance in either procedure. A relational concept, that of generalized
identity matching, was retested after approximately 10 years. The sea lion had originally received trial-and-error exemplar
training with identity matching-to-sample problems prior to transferring the concept to novel stimulus configurations. In
the 10-year memory test, the sea lion immediately and reliably applied the previously established identity concept to familiar
and novel sets of matching problems. These are the first reports of long-term conceptual memory in a nonprimate species. The
experimental findings are consistent with a variety of observations of sea lions in natural settings, which indicate that
natal sites, feeding areas, and individuals may be remembered over long periods of time. Electronic Supplementary Material is available if you access this article at http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-002-0153-8. On that page (frame on the left side),
a link takes you directly to the supplementary material.
Electronic Publication 相似文献
80.
This study examined the role of global processing speed in mediating age increases in auditory memory span in 5- to 13-year-olds. Children were tested on measures of memory span, processing speed, single-word speech rate, phonological sensitivity, and vocabulary. Structural equation modeling supported a model in which age-associated increases in processing speed predicted the availability of long-term memory phonological representations for redintegration processes. The availability of long-term phonological representations, in turn, explained variance in memory span. Maximum speech rate did not predict independent variance in memory span. 相似文献