首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2388篇
  免费   457篇
  国内免费   213篇
  3058篇
  2024年   5篇
  2023年   42篇
  2022年   58篇
  2021年   72篇
  2020年   156篇
  2019年   156篇
  2018年   146篇
  2017年   164篇
  2016年   146篇
  2015年   128篇
  2014年   131篇
  2013年   536篇
  2012年   78篇
  2011年   128篇
  2010年   74篇
  2009年   97篇
  2008年   100篇
  2007年   101篇
  2006年   77篇
  2005年   66篇
  2004年   65篇
  2003年   48篇
  2002年   40篇
  2001年   32篇
  2000年   33篇
  1999年   20篇
  1998年   19篇
  1997年   22篇
  1996年   17篇
  1995年   19篇
  1994年   16篇
  1993年   21篇
  1992年   14篇
  1991年   12篇
  1990年   8篇
  1989年   15篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   8篇
  1986年   12篇
  1985年   16篇
  1984年   15篇
  1983年   7篇
  1982年   10篇
  1981年   14篇
  1980年   14篇
  1979年   17篇
  1978年   16篇
  1977年   29篇
  1976年   17篇
  1975年   12篇
排序方式: 共有3058条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
961.
The present study used a within-participants design to (a) assess the predictive validity of prototype identification versus intention for adolescents’ health behaviours and (b) examine whether control of health behaviour by intention relative to identification is associated with key individual difference variables. Participants were school children (N = 136) who completed measures of intention, perceived behavioural control and prototype identification for 14 health-related behaviours at Time 1, and reported their behaviour 2 weeks later (Time 2). A hierarchical regression showed that prototype identification and intention exhibited similar predictive validity in the prediction of adolescents’ health behaviour. Importantly, identification contributed an additional 6% to the variance in behaviour, after controlling for intention and perceived behavioural control from the theory of planned behaviour [TPB: Ajzen, I. (1991 Ajzen, I. 1991. The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50: 179211. [Crossref], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.]. Additional analyses showed that greater social comparison tendencies, lower agreeableness, greater intellect and less emotional stability were all related to greater control of behaviour by prototype identification. The theoretical and practical implications of these findings are discussed.  相似文献   
962.
Background: Many women experience childbirth as traumatic and 2% develop post-traumatic stress disorder. This study examined the role of health practitioner support and personal control during birth as predictors of post-traumatic stress (PTS) symptoms, adjusting for vulnerability factors of prior trauma, depression, control beliefs and birth intervention. It also investigated interactions between support, prior trauma and birth intervention and their association with PTS symptoms.

Methods: A prospective longitudinal survey of 138 women recruited from UK NHS maternity clinics. Measures were taken in pregnancy, 3 weeks and 3 months after the birth.

Results: Support and control during birth were not predictive of postnatal PTS symptoms. However, support was predictive of PTS symptoms in a subset of women with prior trauma (β?=??0.41, R 2?=?16%) at both 3-weeks and 3-months postpartum. The interaction of birth intervention and support was associated with PTS symptoms 3 months after birth, the relationship between support and PTS symptoms was stronger in women experiencing more intervention.

Conclusions: Low support from health practitioners is predictive of postnatal PTS symptoms in women who have a history of trauma. Longer term effects of low support on postnatal PTS symptoms are also found in women who had more intervention during birth.  相似文献   
963.
Evidence from past studies indicates that children with traumatic brain injury experience difficulties with inhibitory control. Less is known about inhibitory control in children with frontal brain injury related to cerebral infarction. We compared the inhibitory performance of children with frontal infarcts related to sickle cell disease with that of a control group of children with sickle cell disease but no history of cerebral infarction. On a stimulus-response reversal task, children with frontal infarcts made significantly more accuracy errors in the inhibitory condition than controls. Findings from this study and from previous research suggest that impairments in inhibitory control are common following frontal injury in a range of pediatric populations.  相似文献   
964.
The Stroop Interference Test is widely used to assess the inhibition function; however, divergent results have emerged from meta-analyses in children with ADHD. This has led to conflicting results as to whether the Stroop test detects the level of inhibition in these children. We hypothesized that the general approach to include interference scores depending on response time causes conflicting results, whereas recordings of errors may prove a superior measure of the inhibition function in children with ADHD. In the present study, 39 children with an ADHD diagnosis, two subgroups with and without another comorbid mental health disorder, were compared with respect to their interference scores of response time and errors with two subgroups of children with no ADHD. The two subgroups comprised 33 children with another mental health disorder other than ADHD and 56 children with no psychiatric disorder. The between-group analyses detected a multivariate, marginal main effect of an ADHD diagnosis on the Stroop interference scores, and a univariate main effect of an ADHD diagnosis on the interference score of errors. Further, only the interference score of errors predicted significantly the parent reported scores on the Inhibit scale from the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. These findings support that a Stroop interference score of errors is sensitive for inhibition problems in children with ADHD and encourages the use of Stroop versions including error recordings independent of response time.  相似文献   
965.
Research and theory suggest that deficits in trait positive affectivity (PA) and in capacity for executive control of attention (i.e., attentional control or AC) may each intensify risk for depressive symptoms associated with high trait negative affectivity (NA). In contrast, high levels of PA and AC should protect against that risk. However, prospective tests of such predictions are rare. Furthermore, if PA and AC both modulate NA-related risk, it remains to be seen if those effects are independent and complementary or if they operate in an overlapping manner. This study assessed baseline temperament and change in depressed mood across one month in a sample of 125 adolescents. Results supported both PA and AC as modulators of NA’s association with changes in depressed mood. Furthermore, results suggested that these modulating effects are largely non-overlapping and complementary, such that when both PA and AC are low, high NA predicts increases in depressed mood. In contrast, high NA predicts decreases in depressed mood when both PA and AC are high. Limitations of these findings, directions for future research, and implications for prevention are discussed.  相似文献   
966.
Identifying risk factors for the development of internalizing disorders is of major importance. In this context, behavioral inhibition (BIS), the fight-flight-freeze-system (FFFS), and attentional control (AC) have been proposed as being possible risk factors for both anxiety disorders and depression. Even though related cross-sectionally, it is still unclear whether these factors are precursors for internalizing disorders. In this longitudinal study, 1811 participants (aged 12–15) completed questionnaires on BIS/FFFS, AC, and internalizing symptoms at pretest and at two year follow-up. Supporting the alleged importance of BIS/FFFS and AC in the development of internalizing symptoms, BIS/FFFS and AC showed predictive value for anxiety and depression symptoms at two year follow-up. For anxiety symptoms this predictive value was not independent of the level of symptoms at pretest. For depression symptoms, AC showed predictive value over and above pretest level of depression symptoms. In the context of early detection of at risk adolescents, results suggest that screening of current anxiety and depression symptoms is most relevant in addition to a focus on AC for the screening of depression. However, it cannot be ruled out that at a further extended follow-up also BIS/FFFS might show independent predictive value for symptoms of anxiety and/or depression.  相似文献   
967.
RESUMEN

El entrenamiento en habilidades sociales y el programa de terapia familiar están siendo evaluados en un proyecto de investigación que incluye un tratamiento comparativo con terapia de movilización. En este tipo de terapia los pacientes, asignados al azar y con una esquizofrenia documentada, practican relajación, footing, reducción del estrés estrés, actividades manuales y terapia creativa. Se ha intentado en este artículo hacer una primera aproximación a un enfoque de tratamiento psicosocial de la esquizofrenia con base empírica. Bajo la optimista pretensión teórica de que los pacientes esquizofrénicos tratados tienen capacidad para aprender; que las actitudes y sentimientos familiares pueden cambiar en forma positiva y el ambiente donde se desenvuelven puede diseñarse para suspender la diátesis, mediatizada biológicamente.  相似文献   
968.
《Estudios de Psicología》2013,34(2):231-249
Resumen

Se propone un bosquejo de teoría para el estudio del fenómeno de la consciencia desde un punto de vista unificador—natural y artificial—basado en el análisis de la mente como un fenómeno de control. Se incluye una justificación de la investigación en consciencia artificial no como imitación mecánica de la consciencia natural, sino por las propiedades de que puede dotar a un sistema técnico. Se describe la evolución no biomimética de las estructuras básicas de control y se analizan las estrategias más actuales para el desarrollo de máquinas conscientes.  相似文献   
969.
《Estudios de Psicología》2013,34(51):99-126
Resumen

Los tres paradigmas más importantes desarrollados en el área del aprendizaje implícito son revisados a la luz de la definición recientemente planteada por Reber (1993) d este conocimiento como aquél que se produce independientemente de los intentos conscientes por aprender, y en ausencia de un reconocimiento explícito de sus resultados. Se repasan los estudios empíricos más relevantes llevados a cabo siguiendo cada uno de estos tres paradigmas, y se discute el grado en que podría asumirse para cada uno de ellos el cumplimiento de estos requisitos, concluyendo que sería el paradigma de aprendizaje secuencial el único en el que se habría demostrado suficientemente la existencia de un proceso implícito de aprendizaje. Los recientes intentos de elaborar modelos computacionales de los mecanismos responsables de estos aprendizajes se señalan, por último, como un enfoque alternativo a este mismo problema de delimitación de los procesos de aprendizaje asociativo no consciente, que podrían ayudar a comprender su naturaleza y limitaciones, analizando el modo como la complejidad podría derivarse, en el sistema cognitivo humano, a partir de procesos computacionales más simples, inferiores al nivel de elaboración de la conciencia.  相似文献   
970.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号