首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   509篇
  免费   56篇
  国内免费   36篇
  2024年   8篇
  2023年   17篇
  2022年   16篇
  2021年   33篇
  2020年   32篇
  2019年   26篇
  2018年   53篇
  2017年   47篇
  2016年   47篇
  2015年   25篇
  2014年   31篇
  2013年   121篇
  2012年   7篇
  2011年   18篇
  2010年   10篇
  2009年   22篇
  2008年   25篇
  2007年   9篇
  2006年   11篇
  2005年   6篇
  2004年   6篇
  2003年   1篇
  2002年   8篇
  2001年   4篇
  2000年   2篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   1篇
  1997年   1篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   1篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   2篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   1篇
  1980年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
排序方式: 共有601条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
241.
To describe trajectories of aggressive behaviors for adolescents living in rural areas, we compared the patterns, timing and sex differences in development of physical and social aggression using five waves of data collected from youth in school surveys administered over 2.5 years. The sample (N = 5,151) was 50.0% female, 52.1% Caucasian and 38.2% African-American. Multilevel growth curve models showed that physical and social aggression followed curvilinear trajectories from ages 11 to 18, with increases in each type of aggression followed by subsequent declines. Physical aggression peaked around age 15; social aggression peaked around age 14. Boys consistently perpetrated more physical aggression than girls, but the trajectories were parallel. There were no sex differences in the perpetration of social aggression. Given the characteristics of the developmental trajectories observed, interventions with both boys and girls targeting physically and socially aggressive behaviors are needed in early adolescence to slow the development of aggression.  相似文献   
242.
Forgiveness is often assumed to be adaptive for psychological adjustment following interpersonal transgressions. Three hundred and forty seven individuals who had experienced a recent interpersonal transgression were surveyed on four occasions over the course of six weeks. Forgiveness was assessed with scales measuring interpersonal avoidance and revenge motivation and psychological adjustment was assessed with scales measuring depression and rumination. Latent growth curve analyses showed that intraindividual changes in forgiveness were positively correlated with changes in adjustment. Latent difference score analyses indicated that adjustment predicted subsequent change in forgiveness, but that forgiveness did not predict subsequent change in adjustment. The results suggest that adjustment facilitates forgiveness, but not that forgiveness facilitates adjustment.  相似文献   
243.
Latent growth mixture modeling was used to identify discrete patterns of physical aggression from Grades 7 to 11 among a sample of 1,877 youth. Four trajectory classes adequately explained the development of physical aggression in both boys and girls: Low/No Aggression; Persistent High Aggression; Desisting Aggression, characterized by decreasing risk throughout adolescence; and Adolescent Aggression, characterized by low early risk that increases until Grade 9, levels out, and then declines in late adolescence. Girls were less likely than boys were to be in any trajectory besides the Low/No Aggression trajectory. Parental supervision, deviant peer association, academic orientation, impulsivity, and emotional distress at Grade 7 were all strongly associated with trajectory class membership. These associations did not differ by gender. These findings strongly suggest that the processes involved in the development of physical aggression in adolescence operate similarly in boys and girls.  相似文献   
244.
宫内生长受限分娩时间决策   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
宫内生长受限(IUGR)是导致围产儿死亡的常见原因之一。分娩时间是IUGR临床决策的关键问题,IUGR分娩时间的临床决策需对其病因、病理生理、健康效益、医疗代价、产前检测等进行辩证综合分析,采用循证医学的方法,科学决策,个体化决策,力求减少不良后果发生。  相似文献   
245.
246.
This study examined the effects of passage and presentation order on progress monitoring assessments of oral reading fluency in 134 second grade students. The students were randomly assigned to read six one-minute passages in one of six fixed orders over a seven week period. The passages had been developed to be comparable based on readability formulas. Estimates of oral reading fluency varied across the six stories (67.9 to 93.9), but not as a function of presentation order. These passage effects altered the shape of growth trajectories and affected estimates of linear growth rates, but were shown to be removed when forms were equated. Explicit equating is essential to the development of equivalent forms, which can vary in difficulty despite high correlations across forms and apparent equivalence through readability indices.  相似文献   
247.
We investigated the effects of grade retention in first grade on the growth of the Woodcock-Johnson broad mathematics and reading scores over three years using linear growth curve modeling on an academically at-risk sample. A large sample (n = 784) of first grade children who were at risk for retention was initially identified based on low literacy scores. Scores representing propensity for retention were constructed based on 72 variables collected in comprehensive baseline testing in first grade. We closely matched 97 pairs of retained and promoted children based on their propensity scores using optimal matching procedures. This procedure adjusted for baseline differences between the retained and promoted children. We found that grade retention decreased the growth rate of mathematical skills but had no significant effect on reading skills. In addition, several potential moderators of the effect of retention on growth of mathematical and reading skills were identified including limited English language proficiency and children's conduct problems.  相似文献   
248.
孙赫 《心理学探新》2009,29(6):16-22
"生命需要心理学理论与生命教育方法"是研究者开展了为期五年的生命发展研究后获得的心理学与教育学的研究成果。五年中,研究者以0~6岁正常学前儿童为对象,对其实行生命发展教育,获取了大量的生命发展事实,证明了:1)人类生命具有人人可遵循的生长发展规律——"需要"的生长发展规律;2)"需要"的生物性基础的早期建构是"需要"生长发展规律的核心内容;3)马斯洛的"需要层次"中心理需要的低级阶段——"安全需要"之前还存在一个生命的原始需要——以"新奇、探索、操作"为对象的"需要",其贯穿在0~6岁的儿童早期生命阶段;4)0~6岁时期,"儿童需要"不是恒定的、稳定的、成熟的心理机能,而是处在生长过程中。"需要"强度的提升是0~6岁时期以"需要"为核心的心理整体建构的鲜为人知的生命法则;5)0~6岁时期"需要"的良好建构,使"需要"水平达到推动生命走向"需要"的最高层次——"自我实现需要"的强度;6)"需要层次"与生命质量存在着内在关联;7)儿童"原始需要"满足的方法为:在儿童"原始需要"机能强度与寓含"新奇、探索、操作"属性的"原始需要"对象即外部刺激之间建立起犹如昆虫与花类般默契的真正联系。  相似文献   
249.
Martin Seligman, in his very popular book Authentic Happiness (Seligman 2002), argued that authentic happiness is derived from three major sets of experiences in life, namely experiencing pleasantness regularly (the pleasant life), experiencing a high level of engagement in satisfying activities (the engaged life), and experiencing a sense of connectedness to a greater whole (the meaningful life). In this paper, we maintain that balance in life contributes significantly to subjective well-being. Balance contributes to subjective well-being because of the satisfaction limit that people can derive from a single life domain. People have to be involved in multiple domains to satisfy the full spectrum of human development needs. Different life domains tend to focus on different human developmental needs. More specifically, balance contributes to subjective well-being because subjective well-being can only be attained when both survival and growth needs are met. High levels of subjective well-being cannot be attained with satisfaction of basic needs or growth needs alone. Both needs have to be met to induce subjective well-being.  相似文献   
250.
A widespread belief in happiness research is that measures of life satisfaction capture the variety of an individual’s experiences along a single favorable—unfavorable dimension. The current article points to a possible violation of this assumption. With a combination of data and theory it argues that life satisfaction is not a balanced reflection of subjective experiences because the evaluation that drives judgments of life satisfaction is tilted towards pleasure and away from engagement. We postulate that feelings of pleasure are overrated in statements of life satisfaction, whereas feelings of engagement are underrepresented. Feelings of engagement and interest are further supposed to be indicators of personal growth. Measures of personal growth and life satisfaction should thus be unrelated. Empirical evidence for our model is provided by self-report and reaction time data from a Norwegian convenience sample (N = 30). Detection latencies for pleasant stimuli correlated significantly with life satisfaction but not with measures of personal growth. Life satisfaction correlated significantly with pleasant experiences, but not with engagement. Personal growth correlated significantly with feelings of engagement. Life satisfaction and personal growth were uncorrelated.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号