全文获取类型
收费全文 | 10334篇 |
免费 | 815篇 |
国内免费 | 200篇 |
专业分类
11349篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 6篇 |
2024年 | 91篇 |
2023年 | 125篇 |
2022年 | 119篇 |
2021年 | 306篇 |
2020年 | 451篇 |
2019年 | 857篇 |
2018年 | 685篇 |
2017年 | 849篇 |
2016年 | 825篇 |
2015年 | 663篇 |
2014年 | 645篇 |
2013年 | 1173篇 |
2012年 | 449篇 |
2011年 | 435篇 |
2010年 | 337篇 |
2009年 | 384篇 |
2008年 | 366篇 |
2007年 | 359篇 |
2006年 | 346篇 |
2005年 | 290篇 |
2004年 | 250篇 |
2003年 | 230篇 |
2002年 | 206篇 |
2001年 | 175篇 |
2000年 | 150篇 |
1999年 | 112篇 |
1998年 | 57篇 |
1997年 | 52篇 |
1996年 | 36篇 |
1995年 | 42篇 |
1994年 | 47篇 |
1993年 | 30篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 30篇 |
1990年 | 19篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1988年 | 18篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
941.
John Turri Ori Friedman Ashley Keefner 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2017,70(3):504-515
Five experiments (N?=?1710) demonstrate the central role of knowledge attributions in social evaluations. In Experiments 1–3, we manipulated whether an agent believes, is certain of, or knows a true proposition and asked people to rate whether the agent should perform a variety of actions. We found that knowledge, more so than belief or certainty, leads people to judge that the agent should act. In Experiments 4–5, we investigated whether attributions of knowledge or certainty can explain an important finding on how people act based on statistical evidence, known as “the Wells effect”. We found that knowledge attributions, but not certainty attributions, mediate this effect on decision making. 相似文献
942.
Martha Chamodraka Marilyn R. Fitzpatrick Jennifer Ingrid Janzen 《The journal of positive psychology》2017,12(3):232-245
AbstractResearch on the relationship of client hope to successful therapy outcome underlines the need to discover the processes and conditions when hope flourishes. Snyder’s Hope Theory has been applied to psychotherapy but client subjective experiences are needed to illuminate nuances in the process. The present study examined the trajectory of hope development in psychotherapy using interviews with 18 counseling clients, analyzed using grounded theory methodology. Findings were synthesized into a dynamic model titled hope as empowerment model (HEM) that shares features of Snyder’s Hope Theory. HEM introduces patterns of particular salience to psychotherapy. The compatibility of client preferences with therapist input raised faith in the process of counseling and, in turn, hope in a positive outcome. A number of conditions, including the flexibility of client role preferences and the directiveness of hope-inspiring therapeutic strategies, influenced the nature and strength of client hope. Implications for therapy and positive psychology are considered. 相似文献
943.
《International Journal of Applied Psychoanalytic Studies》2017,14(1):22-34
This article discusses critically the focus on stress symptoms when treating traumatized refugees. It describes common challenges that practitioners meet, when working with extreme trauma. It suggests three concepts that might help to understand the complicated dynamics of severe traumatization. These concepts deal as well with the nature of post‐traumatic problems as with the way they might be treated. A clinical case is used to exemplify and discuss the concepts. Finally the article offers some thoughts on the special dynamics of the situation of refugees. 相似文献
944.
《PsyCh Journal》2017,6(3):175-184
The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory‐2 (MMPI ‐2) is often used in forensic psychological/psychiatric assessment. This was a pilot study on the utility of the Chinese MMPI ‐2 in detecting feigned mental disorders. The sample consisted of 194 university students who were either simulators (informed or uninformed) or controls. All the participants were administered the Chinese MMPI ‐2 and the Structured Interview of Reported Symptoms‐2 (SIRS ‐2). The results of the SIRS ‐2 were utilized to classify the participants into the feigning or control groups. The effectiveness of eight detection indices was investigated by using item analysis, multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA ), and receiver operating characteristic (ROC ) analysis. Results indicated that informed‐simulating participants with prior knowledge of mental disorders did not perform better in avoiding feigning detection than uninformed‐simulating participants. In addition, the eight detection indices of the Chinese MMPI ‐2 were effective in discriminating participants in the feigning and control groups, and the best cut‐off scores of three of the indices were higher than those obtained from the studies using the English MMPI ‐2. Thus, in this sample of university students, the utility of the Chinese MMPI ‐2 in detecting feigned mental disorders was tentatively supported, and the Chinese Infrequency Scale (ICH ), a scale developed specifically for the Chinese MMPI ‐2, was also supported as a valid scale for validity checking. 相似文献
945.
《Thought: A Journal of Philosophy》2017,6(3):147-156
Ambivalence—where we experience two conflicting emotional responses to the same object, person or state of affairs—is sometimes thought to pose a problem for cognitive theories of emotion. Drawing on the ideas of the Stoic Chrysippus, I argue that a cognitivist can account for ambivalence without retreating from the view that emotions involve fully‐fledged evaluative judgments. It is central to the account I offer that emotions involve two kinds of judgment: one about the object of emotion, and one about the subject's response. 相似文献
946.
947.
Chen-Wei Liu Robert Philip Chalmers 《The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology》2021,74(1):118-138
Using Louis’ formula, it is possible to obtain the observed information matrix and the corresponding large-sample standard error estimates after the expectation–maximization (EM) algorithm has converged. However, Louis’ formula is commonly de-emphasized due to its relatively complex integration representation, particularly when studying latent variable models. This paper provides a holistic overview that demonstrates how Louis’ formula can be applied efficiently to item response theory (IRT) models and other popular latent variable models, such as cognitive diagnostic models (CDMs). After presenting the algebraic components required for Louis’ formula, two real data analyses, with accompanying numerical illustrations, are presented. Next, a Monte Carlo simulation is presented to compare the computational efficiency of Louis’ formula with previously existing methods. Results from these presentations suggest that Louis’ formula should be adopted as a standard method when computing the observed information matrix for IRT models and CDMs fitted with the EM algorithm due to its computational efficiency and flexibility. 相似文献
948.
Luca Stefanutti Andrea Spoto 《The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology》2021,74(3):465-486
In recent years a number of articles have focused on the identifiability of the basic local independence model. The identifiability issue usually concerns two model parameter sets predicting an identical probability distribution on the response patterns. Both parameter sets are applied to the same knowledge structure. However, nothing is known about cases where different knowledge structures predict the same probability distribution. This situation is referred to as ʻempirical indistinguishabilityʼ between two structures and is the main subject of the present paper. Empirical indistinguishability is a stronger form of unidentifiability, which involves not only the parameters, but also the structural and combinatorial properties of the model. In particular, as far as knowledge structures are concerned, a consequence of empirical indistinguishability is that the existence of certain knowledge states cannot be empirically established. Most importantly, it is shown that model identifiability cannot guarantee that a certain knowledge structure is empirically distinguishable from others. The theoretical findings are exemplified in a number of different empirical scenarios. 相似文献
949.
Jerome Anthony Lewis Zachary M. Himmelberger J. Dean Elmore 《International journal of psychology》2021,56(5):710-715
Many studies indicate that increasing self-awareness leads to individuals reflecting on their values and ideals (Silvia & Duval, 2001). This self-reflection appears to increase prosocial behaviour (Berkowitz, 1987). However, previously studies typically manipulated self-awareness in situations in which the individual may have felt pressure from the researcher to help. Thus, experimenter pressure to behave prosocially confounds the self-awareness explanation provided in past research. We used a novel experimental paradigm to manipulate self-awareness and remove the researcher's presence to decrease the likelihood that the participant would conform to experimenter demand. Participants were 36 college students (Mage = 19.52; 25 women). The results indicated a strong probability that the experimental condition participants were more prosocial than control condition participants. These findings provide additional support for the hypothesis that self-reflection increases prosocial behaviour, even without experimenter demands. These findings and the importance of studying objective self-awareness in light of the coronavirus are discussed. 相似文献
950.
The primary objective of this study was to contribute to the growing research discipline investigating the effects of physical exercise on divergent thinking creativity performance. Thirty‐two students participated in this two‐visit, within‐subject intervention. Individuals consented to participate in two randomized, and counterbalanced, experimental conditions, consisting of 15 min of active treadmill walking and an inert, seated control incubation period. Creativity was assessed at baseline and post‐exercise (and control) via the Instances Creativity Task (ICT). Creativity scores for changes in fluency (F(1, 31) = 2.90, p = .10) were not statistically significant across the experimental conditions. Originality scores were higher at baseline and follow‐up when compared to the exercise condition (F(1, 31) = 6.82, p = .01). However, there was no statistically significant condition × time interaction effect (F(1, 31) = 1.78, p = .19). Further analyses demonstrated that there was no statistically significant difference between the experimental conditions on recall score (F(1, 31) = 1.04, p = .32). All models indicated statistically significant main effects for time: fluency (F(1, 31) = 131.17, p < .001); originality (F(1, 31) = 36.54, p < .001); and recall (F(1, 31) = 51.75, p < .001). These findings suggest that both active and inert creative incubation periods may similarly enhance subsequent divergent thinking performance. 相似文献