全文获取类型
收费全文 | 163篇 |
免费 | 19篇 |
国内免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
193篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 2篇 |
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 2篇 |
2018年 | 9篇 |
2017年 | 13篇 |
2016年 | 13篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 15篇 |
2013年 | 25篇 |
2012年 | 3篇 |
2011年 | 6篇 |
2010年 | 3篇 |
2009年 | 6篇 |
2008年 | 11篇 |
2007年 | 9篇 |
2006年 | 7篇 |
2005年 | 11篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 5篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有193条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Joseph?CiarrochiEmail author Hank?Robb Claire?Godsell 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》2005,23(2):79-106
In recent years, a new “wave” of mindfulness based Cognitive Behavior Therapies (CBT) has become popular. Such approaches include Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT; Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behaviour change. New York: Guilford Press, 1999), Mindfulness-based Cognitive Therapy for Depression (Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: A new approach to preventing relapse. New York: Guilford Press, 2002), and Mindfulness-based Stress Management (Full catastrophe living: using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Dell Publishing, 1990). In contrast to traditional CBT, these approaches often minimize attempts to change the form and frequency of dysfunctional thoughts. Is there any way to integrate traditional CBT with mindfulness based CBT? To answer this question, we discuss the philosophical and theoretical underpinnings of one form of traditional CBT (Rational-Emotive and Cognitive Behavioral Therapy) and one form of mindfulness based CBT (ACT). We argue that some aspects of each therapy can be integrated. However, in order to prevent techniques from being used haphazardly or inconsistently, we suggest that the different forms of CBT need to be driven by a common philosophical orientation (e.g., functional contextualism) and theoretical orientation (e.g., Relational Frame Theory). 相似文献
72.
本实验探讨了对置于正方形、等边三角形和圆中的刺激点所进行的1点定位、2点定位、3点定位,并比较了在定位有框架和定位无框架时的情况。结果发现:(1)在刺激呈现时间为100ms和300ms时.在3种定位任务中,当定位有框架时被试定位的绝对误差均小于定位无框架的。(2)在两种呈现时间下被试在3种定位任务中的绝对误差均处于同一水平,表现出现了视觉定位中的结构效应,并且几个不同的框架没有不同的作用。 相似文献
73.
Claudio Pizzi 《Studia Logica》2007,85(3):395-410
The paper introduces a contingential language extended with a propositional constant τ axiomatized in a system named KΔτ ,
which receives a semantical analysis via relational models. A definition of the necessity operator in terms of Δ and τ allows
proving (i) that KΔτ is equivalent to a modal system named K□τ (ii) that both KΔτ and K□τ are tableau-decidable and complete with respect to the defined relational semantics (iii) that the modal τ -free fragment of KΔτ is exactly
the deontic system KD. In §4 it is proved that the modal τ -free fragment of a system KΔτw weaker than KΔτ is exactly the
minimal normal system K.
Presented by Daniele Mundici 相似文献
74.
Past research has demonstrated a transformation of stimulus functions under similar conditions using gambling tasks and adults (e.g., Zlomke & Dixon, 2006), and the present study attempted to extend this research. Experimenters exposed 7 children (ages 7 to 10 years) to a simulated board game with concurrently available dice differing only by color. Following initial exposure to the game, participants were trained to discriminate between two contextual cues representing the relational frames of more than and less than. Following the training procedure, experimenters reexposed participants to the simulated board game. Six of the 7 participants demonstrated an increased preference toward the die with the color that had been paired with more than during the conditional discrimination training. 相似文献
75.
Sirois F 《The International journal of psycho-analysis》2008,89(4):759-771
Transference symptom is a hazy notion in Freud's writings. The notion is presented here as a particular moment in the crystallization of the transference neurosis. It results from a double cathexis of the analytic frame and the analyst resulting in a symbolic distortion that is represented plastically within the session, as occurs in dreams. The transference symptom proceeds from two different preconscious cathexes, one attached to the reality of the frame, the other to the drive linked to the analyst. A psychic space is thereby opened up for interpreting both the resistance and the unconscious derivatives of infantile conflict. The transference symptom is a compromise formation that includes the analyst and questions the countertransference stance. Three different analytic situations give rise to transference symptoms according to the relative balance between frame and process in the analytic encounter. The concept is compared with enactment. 相似文献
76.
77.
Becky L. Nastally Mark R. Dixon James W. Jackson 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2010,43(1):125-129
Pathological and nonpathological gamblers completed a task that assessed preference among 2 concurrently available slot machines. Subsequent assessments of choice were conducted after various attempts to transfer contextual functions associated with irrelevant characteristics of the slot machines. Results indicated that the nonproblem gambling group, but not the problem gambling group, increased their responding toward the slot initially trained as greater than following the initial training procedure, then decreased their responding toward that slot following the reversal phase. 相似文献
78.
79.
Otto F. Kernberg 《Scandinavian Psychoanalytic Review》2013,36(1):2-21
The concept of omnipotence refers to a primitive fantasy, a mechanism of defense, and a pathological psychic structure. Omnipotence and its derivative defensive operation, omnipotent control, are highly prevalent in borderline personality organization. Three clinical vignettes illustrate these mechanisms in the treatment of patients with borderline, narcissistic, and obsessive personality disorders, respectively. These vignettes illustrate the transference developments when omnipotence and omnipotence control are dominant, and the therapeutic approach to these conditions. 相似文献
80.
The present study investigated how trait anxiety influenced the formation of a self‐frame and decision making. Participants (N = 1044) responded to the Trait Anxiety Inventory. Those with trait anxiety scores ±1 Z score from the sample mean (N = 328) were recalled to respond to the self‐frame questionnaire. The results suggested that trait anxiety differences could result in differences in the editing of decision‐making information, thereby influencing the risky choice. Compared with the low trait anxiety group, participants from the high trait anxiety group showed a greater tendency to use negative vocabulary to construct their self‐frame and tended to choose conservative plans. Self‐frame suppressed the influence of trait anxiety on decision making. These results further confirmed the hypothesis that individual differences in personality traits might influence the processing of information in a framed decision task. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献