全文获取类型
收费全文 | 70篇 |
免费 | 20篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
98篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 3篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 3篇 |
2016年 | 5篇 |
2015年 | 2篇 |
2014年 | 2篇 |
2013年 | 12篇 |
2012年 | 6篇 |
2011年 | 3篇 |
2010年 | 2篇 |
2009年 | 2篇 |
2008年 | 3篇 |
2007年 | 2篇 |
2006年 | 2篇 |
2005年 | 6篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 1篇 |
2002年 | 1篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 1篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有98条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
Bernt Ivar Olsen Bruno Laeng Kari‐Ann Kristiansen Gunnar Hartvigsen 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2013,54(3):196-204
Performance in a computerized “mental rotation” task was measured in groups of males and females while they rotated Shepard‐Metzler‐like cube assemblies on either a standard laptop screen (size = 36 cm) or on a large display wall (584 cm) where the stimuli appeared at considerably larger sizes and within a much wider field of view than that typically used in most spatial tasks. Males and females did not differ significantly in performance in the standard size condition with regards to response time but females performed faster than males in the large display condition. Males were also found to be significantly more accurate than females, regardless of display. We found no sign of trading accuracy for speed for either of the sexes or screen size conditions. We surmise that such an effect may be due to differences in task‐solving strategies between the sexes, where a holistic strategy – which may be preferred by males is negatively affected by large object sizes, whereas a piecemeal approach, that may be preferred by females, is virtually unaffected by display size. 相似文献
12.
Belinda Campos Michelle N. Shiota Dacher Keltner Gian C. Gonzaga Jennifer L. Goetz 《Cognition & emotion》2013,27(1):37-52
Understanding positive emotions' shared and differentiating features can yield valuable insight into the structure of positive emotion space and identify emotion states, or aspects of emotion states, that are most relevant for particular psychological processes and outcomes. We report two studies that examined core relational themes (Study 1) and expressive displays (Study 2) for eight positive emotion constructs—amusement, awe, contentment, gratitude, interest, joy, love, and pride. Across studies, all eight emotions shared one quality: high positive valence. Distinctive core relational theme and expressive display patterns were found for four emotions—amusement, awe, interest, and pride. Gratitude was associated with a distinct core relational theme but not an expressive display. Joy and love were each associated with a distinct expressive display but their core relational themes also characterised pride and gratitude, respectively. Contentment was associated with a distinct expressive display but not a core relational theme. The implications of this work for the study of positive emotion are discussed. 相似文献
13.
《The journal of positive psychology》2013,8(3):234-236
No research to date has supported the ‘social contagion’ effect of laughter and smiles in a non-Western context, nor among children. Expressions of positive affect were here compared according to social and non-social conditions among 163 children between the ages of 5–12 while playing in a park in Afghanistan. Independent observers coded laugh and smile responses using published criteria and identified 147 laugh responses and 697 smile responses. Children in the social play condition demonstrated significantly more laughs than children in the non-social play condition, and the number of peer observers present in the social play condition significantly increased the number of participant smile responses, but not laugh responses. Findings supported the idea in Western research that laughter and smiles may function in moderating social relationships and communicating affect. The authors recommend future research to investigate the relationship between positive affect and well-being across cultures, with consideration for practical interventions. 相似文献
14.
15.
16.
Substructural logics on display 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
17.
Does the perception of our actions differ from the perception of other individuals' actions when we observe them, like other individual's actions, in an offline perspective? Previous studies, using recognition as well as prediction judgments, suggest that it does even if the stimulus information is reduced to a single moving point-light. Here, we assessed whether this difference also affects the timing of actions. This was tested in two experiments, using a specific synchronization task. After some practice, self-generated action events were anticipated faster than other action events, provided that the anticipation could not be accomplished sufficiently well on the basis of easily detectable cues. The results are discussed with regard to the previous findings of off-line authorship effects in action perception. 相似文献
18.
儿童情绪伪装能力的发展研究 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:1
该文介绍了国外儿童情绪伪装能力发展及其影响因素的近期研究,这些研究结果表明:儿童最早在斗岁出现情绪伪装的认知能力,6岁时这种能力得到完善;儿童对情绪伪装认知能力和对物理属性的表面与真实的区分能力具有潜在的一致的认知基础;儿童情绪伪装能力的完善和错误信念的获得有关;文化通过社会化过程影响儿童的情绪伪装的发展。 相似文献
19.
20.
用追踪研究方法,对60名幼儿在高兴、惊讶与害怕情境中的情绪表达规则认知两年的发展进行探讨,并考察情绪情境、性别和人际背景对其发展的影响。结果表明:(1)幼儿的情绪表达规则知识、目标和策略水平均随年龄增长显著提升;(2)幼儿在害怕情境中为自我保护而较多使用掩饰策略,在惊讶情境中较多使用掩饰和夸大策略,在高兴情境中综合运用掩饰、弱化和夸大策略;(3)性别和人际背景均对幼儿情绪表达规则认知发展有显著影响。 相似文献