首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   256篇
  免费   59篇
  国内免费   8篇
  2023年   5篇
  2022年   1篇
  2021年   4篇
  2020年   8篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   12篇
  2017年   16篇
  2016年   15篇
  2015年   4篇
  2014年   9篇
  2013年   47篇
  2012年   11篇
  2011年   7篇
  2010年   9篇
  2009年   2篇
  2008年   4篇
  2007年   13篇
  2006年   6篇
  2005年   14篇
  2004年   3篇
  2003年   9篇
  2002年   15篇
  2001年   8篇
  2000年   8篇
  1999年   5篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   7篇
  1995年   5篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   7篇
  1987年   1篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   2篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   2篇
  1980年   4篇
  1979年   3篇
  1978年   4篇
  1977年   3篇
  1976年   6篇
  1974年   1篇
排序方式: 共有323条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
There is much debate as to the extent and nature of functional specialization within the different subregions of the prefrontal cortex. The current study was undertaken to investigate the effect of damage to medial prefrontal cortex subregions in the rat. Rats were trained on two biconditional discrimination tasks, one auditory and one visual, in two different contexts. At test, they received presentations of audiovisual compounds of these training stimuli in extinction. These compounds had dictated either the same (congruent trials) or different (incongruent trials) responses during training. In sham-operated controls, contextual cues came to control responding to conflicting information provided by incongruent stimulus compounds. Experiment 1 demonstrated that this contextual control of responding was not evident in individual rats with large amounts of damage that included the prelimbic and cingulate subregions of the prefrontal cortex. Experiment 2 further dissociated the result of Experiment 1, demonstrating that lesions specific to the anterior cingulate cortex were sufficient to produce a deficit early on during presentation of an incongruent stimulus compound but that performance was unimpaired as presentation progressed. This early deficit suggests a role for the anterior cingulate cortex in the detection of response conflict, and for the medial prefrontal cortex in the contextual control of competing responses, providing evidence for functional specialization within the rat prefrontal cortex.  相似文献   
82.
Dose-dependent changes in sensitivity to reinforcement were found when rats were treated with low, moderate, and high doses of the partial dopamine D1-type receptor agonist SKF38393 and with the nonselective dopamine agonist apomorphine, but did not change when rats were treated with similar doses of the selective dopamine D2-type receptor agonist quinpirole. Estimates of bias did not differ significantly across exposure to SKF38393 or quinpirole, but did change significantly at the high dose of apomorphine. Estimates of goodness of fit (r2) did not change significantly during quinpirole exposure. Poor goodness of fit was obtained for the high doses of SKF38393 and apomorphine. Decrements in absolute rates of responding were observed at the high dose of quinpirole and at the moderate and high doses of SKF38393 and apomorphine. Changes in r2 and absolute responding may be due to increases in stereotyped behavior during SKF38393 and apomorphine exposure that, in contrast to quinpirole, were distant from the response lever. The present data provide evidence that sensitivity to reward is affected more strongly by dopamine D1-like receptors rather than D2-like receptors, consistent with evidence from other studies investigating consummatory dopamine behavior and the tonic/phasic dopamine hypothesis.  相似文献   
83.
Response rates under random-interval schedules are lower when a brief (500 ms) signal accompanies reinforcement than when there is no signal. The present study examined this signaled-reinforcement effect and its relation to resistance to change. In Experiment 1, rats responded on a multiple random-interval 60-s random-interval 60-s schedule, with signaled reinforcement in only one component. Response resistance to alternative reinforcement, prefeeding, and extinction was compared between these components. Lower response rates, and greater resistance to change, occurred in the component with the reinforcement signal. In Experiment 2, response rates and resistance to change were compared after training on a multiple random-interval 60-s random-interval 60-s schedule in which reinforcer delivery was unsignaled in one component and a response-produced uncorrelated stimulus was presented in the other component. Higher response rates and greater resistance to change occurred with the uncorrelated stimulus. These results highlight the significance of considering the effects of an uncorrelated signal when used as a control condition, and challenge accounts of resistance to change that depend solely on reinforcer rate.  相似文献   
84.
85.
Three experiments were conducted to examine the effects of exposure to a poisoned conspecific on subsequent food aversion in rats. In Experiment 1A, rats that had been aversively conditioned to a cocoa-flavored food were exposed to a poisoned conspecific that had eaten the same food. On the subsequent choice test, the animals increased their aversion to that food. These results were reconfirmed in Experiment 1B, in which a cinnamon-flavored food was used as the stimulus. In Experiment 2, subjects were first exposed to a poisoned conspecific and then conditioned to the food which the conspecific had eaten. On the test, they showed no sign of increased aversion to that food.  相似文献   
86.
Errata     
Corrections to:
K. Tanaka and S. Takimoto, "Effects of intrepersonal affect on fairness judgment" published in Japanese Psychological Review , 1997, Vol.39, No. 4
M. Ito et al., "Visual evoked potentails to geometric forms: Effects of spatial orientation" published in Japanese Psychological Review , 1997, Vol.39, No. 4
K. Ono, "Response stereotypy in humans maintained by response-contingent events" published in Japanese Psychological Review , 1997, Vol.39, No. 4  相似文献   
87.
88.
The blind mole rat (Spalax ehrenbergi) is a solitary and aggressive fossorial rodent that inhabits its own individual tunnel system. Mole rats breed mainly in the winter, giving birth to a litter of three to four young on average [Nevo (1961) Mammalia 25:127–143]. Intraspecific interactions between adults occur mostly during the winter, which is the rainy season in Israel; after parturition, interactions occur between the mother and her offspring and between the siblings themselves until dispersal. Until now, the subterranean habitat of mole rats has prevented direct observation in the wild. Thus, there has been no direct evidence for the proximal factors that drive juvenile mole rats out of the maternal tunnel system as they shift from the social life-style of the young to the solitary life-style of the adult. In the present study, female mole rats were caught in the wild with their offspring and brought to the laboratory. The dispersal process of the young from the maternal nest was followed under two housing conditions: (1) restricted housing, simulating the physical conditions under which dispersal is delayed past the usual age, as often occurs during late winter floods in the field in areas of nonporous clay soils, and (2) nonrestricted housing, simulating the physical conditions that allow the young to disperse from their maternal tunnels at any time. In each housing condition, the mothers raised either one or three pups. During pup development, we monitored both maternal and juvenile agonistic variables during intraspecific interactions. Five major behavioural phases were identified during the ontogeny of the mole rat pups. Maternal aggression toward pups increased mainly in the first three phases of pup development, and sibling aggression increased from the third phase until dispersal. Under the restricted housing conditions, there was a delay in both the appearance of aggression in the “family group” as well as in dispersal age compared with the nonrestricted conditions. In the nonrestricted conditions, increased litter size resulted in delay in dispersal age. We suggest that the main proximate factor initiating natal dispersal is increased intolerance and aggression among siblings rather than maternal aggression. Only after the young began dispersing from the maternal tunnels did maternal aggression dramatically increase, thus preventing the offspring from returning. However, following floods, inhibition of aggression may enable the mole rat pups to stay with the family group and to disperse later when the physical conditions permit it. Aggr. Behav. 24:455–470, 1998. © 1998 Wiley-Liss, Inc.  相似文献   
89.
In a series of three studies, we investigated the influence of a selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitor (fluoxetine) on the rough-and-tumble play of juvenile rats. In Experiment 1, both members of eight pairs of solitary-housed juvenile rats received either vehicle, 2.5, 5, or 10 mg/kg fluoxetine in a counterbalanced within-subject design 20 min before being allowed to play for 5 min periods on four successive test days. The 5 and 10 mg/kg pretreatments significantly reduced incidence of pins during play without affecting dorsal contacts. In Experiment 2, one member of each of 19 established play pairs received 5 mg/kg fluoxetine 20 min before play, while the other member received vehicle. Dominant rats showed no reduction in pins as a result of fluoxetine treatment, but subordinate rats who received fluoxetine exhibited significant reductions in pins. Subsequent dyadic analyses indicated that in pairs where the subordinate animal received fluoxetine, dominant animals maintained their pinning advantage over the 10 days of testing, but in pairs where the dominant animals received fluoxetine, this pinning asymmetry diminished. In Experiment 3, we replicated the above procedure with inexperienced play pairs, to control for the effects of prior social learning. Fluoxetine treatment (5 mg/kg) significantly reduced both pins and dorsal contacts in all treated rats. The results indicate that fluoxetine can reduce the playful pins of juvenile rats, but that prior social learning mediates the strength of these effects. © 1996 Wiley-Liss, Inc.  相似文献   
90.
The present experiments were designed to test whether initial excitatory conditioning to a stimulus was obtained, using a within-subject design of the truly random control (TRC) procedure, and to examine the effects of the density of the conditioned stimulus on it. In Experiment 1, the effect of the three levels of CS density (50%, 25%, 20%) was assessed at six different stages of training, using the conditioned suppression of licking in rats. Although all of the groups given the TRC procedure showed evidence of initial excitatory conditioning, it was more marked in the group with the highest CS density than in the other groups. Experiment 2 confirmed the results of Experiment 1, using a between-subject design. The findings are in line with predictions from the Rescorla-Wagner model, and are discussed with reference to other contemporary theories of associative learning.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号