全文获取类型
收费全文 | 10851篇 |
免费 | 778篇 |
国内免费 | 160篇 |
专业分类
11789篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 19篇 |
2024年 | 147篇 |
2023年 | 243篇 |
2022年 | 178篇 |
2021年 | 230篇 |
2020年 | 537篇 |
2019年 | 547篇 |
2018年 | 506篇 |
2017年 | 514篇 |
2016年 | 487篇 |
2015年 | 319篇 |
2014年 | 453篇 |
2013年 | 1447篇 |
2012年 | 253篇 |
2011年 | 351篇 |
2010年 | 310篇 |
2009年 | 409篇 |
2008年 | 503篇 |
2007年 | 500篇 |
2006年 | 489篇 |
2005年 | 406篇 |
2004年 | 357篇 |
2003年 | 278篇 |
2002年 | 238篇 |
2001年 | 159篇 |
2000年 | 130篇 |
1999年 | 135篇 |
1998年 | 108篇 |
1997年 | 86篇 |
1996年 | 63篇 |
1995年 | 76篇 |
1994年 | 58篇 |
1993年 | 46篇 |
1992年 | 41篇 |
1990年 | 20篇 |
1989年 | 28篇 |
1987年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 147篇 |
1984年 | 135篇 |
1983年 | 116篇 |
1982年 | 94篇 |
1981年 | 85篇 |
1980年 | 84篇 |
1979年 | 73篇 |
1978年 | 104篇 |
1977年 | 72篇 |
1976年 | 45篇 |
1975年 | 39篇 |
1974年 | 31篇 |
1973年 | 38篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
Peter Alevizos William DeRisi Robert Liberman Thad Eckman Edward Callahan 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1978,11(2):243-257
The background and development of a multicategory direct observation system, the Behavior Observation Instrument (BOI), is described. This time-sampling procedure for recording the behavior of persons is demonstrated in several treatment settings and the results applied to issues of program evaluation. Elements that have prevented direct observation from being widely adopted, such as costs, manpower, and training requirements, are systematically analyzed. A basic psychometric analysis of the instrument is used to determine optimum frequency and duration of observation intervals as well as observer agreement. The results imply that direct observation methods, once assumed by some to belong to the special province of the single-subject design, can be used to assess the effects of programs on groups of psychiatric clients in an efficient and economic manner. 相似文献
52.
Allison J 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1978,29(3):557-560
Behavior under baseline conditions in which the contingency is absent can shed some light on the individual's performance under a schedule, but is insufficient as a basis for prediction of performance. This insufficiency of the baseline data runs counter to a recent formalization of the relational principle of reinforcement (Donahoe, 1977). A more satisfactory predictive model must incorporate not only the baseline level of the instrumental response and that of the contingent response, but also the schedule requirements, the character of each response in relation to the other, and the behavior required in simply switching from each to the other. 相似文献
53.
Martin R. Adams 《Journal of Fluency Disorders》1978,3(4):265-271
This article presents a further analysis of stuttering as a phonetic transition defect. Three types of phonetic transitions are identified. Two of these occur within words and represent sites where stutterings are readily detectable. The third transition involves movement from the end of one word to the beginning of the next. Spectrographic evidence is provided to show that stuttering also occurs at points of transition of this type. The same data are interpreted relative to their implications for stuttering theory, research, and therapy. 相似文献
54.
Herman H. Spitz 《Intelligence》1978,2(4):371-379
Mentally retarded individuals have relatively greater difficulty in dealing with strategic games and puzzles than with many other tasks. Consequently, it is of interest to find that strategic games have been played since ancient times, are played throughout the world, and that players who excel are frequently considered wise. Hidden within the more obvious differences between cultures, strategic games are an invariant expression of certain universal intellectual traits. 相似文献
55.
56.
A previous study of the habit reversal treatment of nailbiting was extended by using more subjects (97) and a comparison with an alternative method (negative practice). Negative practice reduced nailbiting episodes by about 60% whereas habit reversal reduced nailbiting episodes by about 99% throughout the 5-month follow-up. 相似文献
57.
The purpose of the present study was to examine the effectiveness of a self-control package in enhancing temporal generality of a parent training program. Sixteen mothers and their clinic-referred noncompliant children were assigned to either a parent training alone group, or a parent training plus self-control group. All mother-child dyads were treated individually by teaching the mother to reward compliance and other prosocial behavior, and to use time-out for noncompliance. In addition, mothers who also received self-control training learned to self-monitor their use of their new parenting skills and to reinforce themselves for use of the skill during a 2 month follow-up period. Assessment consisted of four home observations by independent observers prior to treatment, after treatment, and at a 2 month follow-up. The data indicated that the children in the parent training plus self-control group were significantly more compliant and less deviant at the 2 month follow-up than the children in the parent training alone group. Parental behavior did not differ between the two groups at the follow-up. 相似文献
58.
Previous results indicate that people conform more to group judgments when they have received unanimous group support for their opinions than when they have received no support. The present study tested the hypothesis that these differences would be attenuated when future interaction with the group members was anticipated. The 70 male subjects were tested in a Crutchfield-type conformity situation, where prior group support (100 vs 0%) and anticipation of future interaction (alone vs group) were manipulated. Predictions were based on an examination of conformity incentives that considered normative and informational sources of social influence, as well as Hollander's model of idiosyncracy credit. Consistent with predictions, conformity in the 0% prior support condition was greater when subjects anticipated future group interaction than when they did not, whereas the reverse was true in the 100% prior support condition. In addition, conformity was correlated negatively with both confidence in task ability and feelings of group acceptance when future interaction with the group was anticipated. These data suggest that when future group activity is expected, the tendency to reciprocate past treatment from the group is reduced. 相似文献
59.
Sarah C ODowd 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1980,29(1):36-49
A semantic differential study of development of antonym meanings revealed methodological problems (e.g., concept-scale interaction and developmental changes in scale-checking style) not fully considered in previous semantic differential investigations of child language development. Twenty adjectives were rated on 10 scales by subjects from grades K to 5 (approximately 6 through 11 years old) and adults. Analyses by means, polarity ratings, and average sums of squared differences failed to yield consistent developmental trends. Subjects' choices of scale ratings did show clear age-related differences: Grades K and 1 chose extreme ratings almost exclusively; grades 2 through 4 showed an increase in neutral choices and decreases in extreme ratings; grades 4 and 5 and adults chose extreme ratings least often, neutral choices most often, and more intermediate ratings than any younger subjects. Results show that scale choice is a crucial factor in designing semantic differential studies with children, there is a low correlation between mean scores and ratings given by individual children, and shifts in scale-checking style should be considered when interpreting apparent developmental changes in children's semantic differential ratings. 相似文献
60.