全文获取类型
收费全文 | 239篇 |
免费 | 100篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
346篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 15篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 8篇 |
2016年 | 10篇 |
2015年 | 9篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 30篇 |
2012年 | 10篇 |
2011年 | 8篇 |
2010年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 5篇 |
2007年 | 7篇 |
2006年 | 11篇 |
2005年 | 12篇 |
2004年 | 14篇 |
2003年 | 15篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有346条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
141.
We compared the accuracy of momentary time sampling (MTS) and partial interval recording (PIR) in estimating both absolute behavioral levels and relative change. A computer randomly generated runs of pseudobehavior varying in duration and rate and simulated MTS and PIR of each run. Results indicated that when estimating absolute behavioral levels, duration rather than rate should be used as the dependent measure, and MTS is more accurate than PIR. In contrast, PIR is the more sensitive method for detecting relative changes in behavioral levels, although, at high rates, PIR tends to underestimate the degree of change. 相似文献
142.
R. W. Sanson-Fisher A. Desmond Poole John Dunn 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1980,13(3):493-500
The study sought to examine the effects of varying interval length on the representation of data obtained using modified frequency time sampling. A 7-category scale was used to observe reliably the behavior of eight psychiatric inpatients. Using electronic real time recording equipment, it was possible to computer analyze the obtained data at varying interval lengths, the shortest interval being 1.0 seconds. It was found that increasing the interval length had little effect on the percentage of total duration recorded within each behavioral category, suggesting that this is a relatively stable measure of behavior. Percentage total events for each category was less stable with increasing interval lengths. The number of recorded events within each category tended to decrease, while their average durations tended to increase, as a function of increasing the interval length. The data suggest that the current practice of determining interval length in an arbitrary fashion, or on the basis of convention, should be abandoned. Rather, such a decision should be empirically determined for each particular observation scale and subject group. One method by which this might be achieved is presented. 相似文献
143.
Douglas A. Williams Chrissy M. Chubala Amber A. Mather Kenneth W. Johns 《Learning and motivation》2009,40(4):394-407
Appetitive contextual excitation supported by intertrial unconditioned stimuli was more easily overcome by timed conditioned responding in rats using quiet (Experiment 1) rather than noisy (Experiment 2) food pellet deliveries. Head-entry responding in acquisition peaked above the contextual baseline when pellet delivery occurred 10, 30, 60, or 90 s after the onset of the 120-s white-noise conditioned stimulus (CS). Special tests in extinction revealed CS onset and offset were conditioned by pellet delivery at 0 and 120 s, respectively. Responding was not undermined in Experiment 3 when noisy pellet deliveries replaced quiet pellet deliveries. Our results suggest that micro-stimuli occasioned at different times during the CS are vulnerable to overshadowing, but do not lose strength if they are already predictive. 相似文献
144.
Leung JP 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1993,59(2):401-410
College students responded for monetary rewards in two experiments on choice between differentially segmented aperiodic schedules of reinforcement. On a microcomputer, the concurrent chains were simulated as an air-defense video game in which subjects used two radars for detecting and destroying enemy aircraft. To earn more cash-exchangeable points, subjects had to shoot down as many planes as possible within a given period of time. For both experiments, access to one of two radar systems (terminal link) was controlled by a pair of independent concurrent variable-interval 60-s schedules (initial link) with a 4-s changeover delay always in effect. In Experiment 1, the appearance of an enemy aircraft in the terminal link was determined by a variable-interval (15 s or 60 s) schedule or a two-component chained variable-interval schedule of equal duration. Experiment 2 was similar to Experiment 1 except for the segmented schedule, which had three components. Subjects preferred the unsegmented schedule over its segmented counterpart in the conditions with variable-interval 60 s, and preference tended to be more pronounced with more components in the segmented schedule. These findings are compatible with those from previous studies of periodic and aperiodic schedules with pigeons or humans as subjects. 相似文献
145.
人们熟知的零假设显著性检验,受到一次次质疑与辩护,地位并未动摇,报告检验结果仍然是统计分析的习惯做法。不过,其局限性促使研究者探寻更多的统计方法如区间估计、效应量分析、检验力分析等。本文先介绍假设检验与置信区间的关系;然后讨论检验力与两类错误率和效应量的关系;最后在理顺上述统计方法的基础上,提供一个可操作的统计分析流程。 相似文献
146.
Intraclass correlation and Cronbach's alpha are widely used to describe reliability of tests and measurements. Even with Gaussian
data, exact distributions are known only for compound symmetric covariance (equal variances and equal correlations). Recently,
large sample Gaussian approximations were derived for the distribution functions.
New exact results allow calculating the exact distribution function and other properties of intraclass correlation and Cronbach's
alpha, for Gaussian data with any covariance pattern, not just compound symmetry. Probabilities are computed in terms of the
distribution function of a weighted sum of independent chi-square random variables.
NewF approximations for the distribution functions of intraclass correlation and Cronbach's alpha are much simpler and faster
to compute than the exact forms. Assuming the covariance matrix is known, the approximations typically provide sufficient
accuracy, even with as few as ten observations.
Either the exact or approximate distributions may be used to create confidence intervals around an estimate of reliability.
Monte Carlo simulations led to a number of conclusions. Correctly assuming that the covariance matrix is compound symmetric
leads to accurate confidence intervals, as was expected from previously known results. However, assuming and estimating a
general covariance matrix produces somewhat optimistically narrow confidence intervals with 10 observations. Increasing sample
size to 100 gives essentially unbiased coverage. Incorrectly assuming compound symmetry leads to pessimistically large confidence
intervals, with pessimism increasing with sample size. In contrast, incorrectly assuming general covariance introduces only
a modest optimistic bias in small samples. Hence the new methods seem preferable for creating confidence intervals, except
when compound symmetry definitely holds.
An earlier version of this paper was submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the M.S. in Biostatistics, and
also summarized in a presentation at the meetings of the Eastern North American Region of the International Biometric Society
in March, 2001.
Kistner's work was supported in part by NIEHS training grant ES07018-24 and NCI program project grant P01 CA47 982-04. She
gratefully acknowledges the inspiration of A. Calandra's “Scoring formulas and probability considerations” (Psychometrika, 6, 1–9). Muller's work supported in part by NCI program project grant P01 CA47 982-04. 相似文献
147.
《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2013,66(3):408-425
In four experiments, we varied the time between the onset of distracting nonwords and target colour words in a word–word version of the colour–word contingency learning paradigm. Contingencies were created by pairing a distractor nonword more often with one target colour word than with other colour words. A contingency effect corresponds to faster responses to the target colour word on high-contingency trials (i.e., distractor nonword followed by the target colour word with which it appears most often) than on low-contingency trials (i.e., distractor nonword followed by a target colour word with which it appears only occasionally). Roughly equivalent-sized contingency effects were found at stimulus-onset asynchronies (SOAs) of 50, 250, and 450 ms in Experiment 1, and 50, 500, and 1,000 ms in Experiment 2. In Experiment 3, a contingency effect was observed at SOAs of –50, –200, and –350 ms. In Experiment 4, interstimulus interval (ISI) was varied along with SOA, and learning was equivalent for 200-, 700-, and 1,200-ms SOAs. Together, these experiments suggest that the distracting stimulus does not need to be presented in close temporal contiguity with the response to induce learning. Relations to past research on causal judgement and implications for further contingency learning research are discussed. 相似文献
148.
Variable reinforcement schedules are used to arrange the availability of reinforcement following varying response ratios or intervals of time. Random reinforcement schedules are subtypes of variable reinforcement schedules that can be used to arrange the availability of reinforcement at a constant probability across number of responses or time. Generating schedule values for variable and random reinforcement schedules can be difficult. The present article describes the steps necessary to write macros in Microsoft Excel that will generate variable-ratio, variable-interval, variable-time, random-ratio, random-interval, and random-time reinforcement schedule values. 相似文献
149.
The testing effect refers to the retention benefit conferred by prior retrieval of information from memory. Although the testing effect is a robust phenomenon, a common assumption is that reliable memory benefits only emerge after long retention intervals of days or weeks. The present study focused on potential test-induced retention benefits for brief retention intervals on the order of minutes and tens of seconds. Participants in four experiments studied lists of words. Some of the items were subjected to an initial cued recall test, and others were re-presented for additional study. Free recall tests were administered in each experiment following retention intervals ranging from 30 s to 8 min. When initial retrieval practice was successful (Experiments 1 through 3), or feedback compensated for unsuccessful retrieval (Experiment 4), significant testing effects emerged at all retention intervals. Results are discussed in the context of a bifurcated item-distribution model and highlight the importance of initial test performance and the type of analysis employed when examining testing effect data. 相似文献
150.