全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1071篇 |
免费 | 57篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
1135篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 35篇 |
2020年 | 7篇 |
2019年 | 95篇 |
2018年 | 110篇 |
2017年 | 117篇 |
2016年 | 64篇 |
2015年 | 59篇 |
2014年 | 40篇 |
2013年 | 58篇 |
2012年 | 20篇 |
2011年 | 27篇 |
2010年 | 12篇 |
2009年 | 24篇 |
2008年 | 15篇 |
2007年 | 20篇 |
2006年 | 9篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 9篇 |
2003年 | 7篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 39篇 |
1984年 | 33篇 |
1983年 | 29篇 |
1982年 | 32篇 |
1981年 | 28篇 |
1980年 | 37篇 |
1979年 | 43篇 |
1978年 | 30篇 |
1977年 | 23篇 |
1976年 | 21篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有1135条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
211.
Three experiments are described which elaborate some of the conditions under which rats will contact and manipulate a periodically presented retractable lever. Experiment 1 demonstrated that (i) initial manipulative oral and manual contact with the lever was facilitated if the rat had previous experience of food delivery in the experimental chamber; (ii) persistence in contacting the lever on successive presentations was a function of whether food continued to be presented in the experimental environment; and (iii) food satiation significantly reduced the tendency of the rat to contact the lever even though an expectancy of food had previously been established under conditions of food deprivation. Experiment 2 suggested that the tendency to approach and contact the lever was in part a function of the local moment-to-moment conditional probability of food delivery. Experiment 3 found that the probability of contacting the lever was higher during presentation of an auditory CS signaling a high rate of food delivery than during stimuli signaling no food at all. These results are interpreted as suggesting that the food-signaling aspects of an appetitive CS and that CS's ability to generate signal-directed behaviors are experimentall separable properties. 相似文献
212.
It has been repeatedly shown that the left hemisphere (right visual field) is superior to the right hemisphere (LVF) in reading English, a bias possibly due to any or all of three confounded factors: (1) the symbolic nature of the coding system; (2) the analytic requirements of the decoding process; and (3) the phonological associations of the elements. Recent work on reading Japanese ideograms (Kanji) disentangles (1) from (2) and (3), but leaves the latter two confounded. We further disentangle (2) and (3) by examining visual field preference for reading musical chords, representatives of an analytic, nonphonological symbol system. The strong RVF advantage is interpreted as indicating that the left hemisphere is dominant for reading an analytic symbol system that is not phonologically based. We conclude that the left-hemisphere advantage traditionally found for reading phonological symbols is due to their analytic nature in addition to any effect due to their linguistic association. 相似文献
213.
WU Guanjun 《Frontiers of Philosophy in China》2016,11(4):633
A powerful discourse centered on China’s civilizational subjectivity has emerged in the Sinophone intellectual world since the early 2000s. Among many promoters of this intellectual trend, Gan Yang, together with his slogan of the “fusion of three traditions,” is indeed most influential. This study employs Lacanian psychoanalytic technics to tackle Gan Yang’s thesis, treating the latter not just an object for textual analysis, but more deeply (and fruitfully), that for psychoanalysis. The findings of the study reveal the presence of a fantasmatic structure framing and guaranteeing Gan’s (sharply inconsistent) vision of China’s civilizational subjectivity. Such fantasmatic formation can be referred to as the “Great Dragon fantasy”—a fantasy about China’s civilizational unity and glory. 相似文献
214.
为探讨慢性不可预见性温和应激(chronic unpredictable mild stress, CUMS)诱发抑郁样行为发生中海马5-羟色胺1A受体(5-hydroxytryptamine receptor 1A, 5-HT1AR)表达与作用, 及其对谷氨酸N-甲基-D-天冬氨酸(N-methyl-D-aspartic acid, NMDA)受体和α-氨基羟甲基异恶唑丙酸(α-amino-3-hydroxy-5- methylisoxazole-4-propionic acid, AMPA)受体的影响。通过建立CUMS动物模型, 给应激抑郁模型大鼠海马微量注射5-HT1A受体激动剂、给正常大鼠海马微量注射5-HT1A受体拮抗剂, 测量大鼠体重变化率, 并采用糖水偏爱测试、旷场实验和悬尾实验等方法对大鼠进行行为学检测, 运用Western blot和ELISA方法检测大鼠海马组织中5-HT1AR和NMDAR和AMPAR的关键亚基的表达以及磷酸化水平。结果显示, 与对照组相比, CUMS组大鼠表现出抑郁样行为, 海马5-HT1AR、AMPA受体的GluR2/3亚基表达及磷酸化明显降低, NMDA受体的NR1和NR2B亚基表达及磷酸化显著增加; 正常大鼠海马微量注射5-HT1A受体拮抗剂WAY100635, 动物行为学表现及AMPA受体、NMDA受体表达及磷酸化水平均与CUMS组相同; 注射5-HT1A受体激动剂8-OH-DPAT能逆转应激诱导的上述改变。以上结果表明, CUMS诱发抑郁样行为与海马5-HT1AR表达下降, AMPAR表达量及磷酸化水平降低, NMDAR表达量及磷酸化水平升高有关。5-HT通过5-HT1AR产生抗抑郁作用。5-HT1AR激动剂抗抑郁作用与降低NMDAR表达量及磷酸化水平, 提高AMPAR表达量及磷酸化水平密切相关。 相似文献
215.
216.
217.
218.
Kathleen Bloom 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1984,38(1):93-102
The theoretical, procedural, analytic, and interpretive components of Poulson's (1983, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36, 471–489) study were considered in an effort to clarify the distinction between social reinforcement and social elicitation. Contrary to Poulson's contention, DRO was not, and cannot by definition be, a control for reinforcement. More importantly, the speciestypical, social nature of the infant probably reflects the collaborative influence of elicitation and reinforcement. By carefully unravelling these two interacting processes, future conditioning studies might specially enhance our knowledge of early social development. 相似文献
219.
Offenses committed by those held in institutions can have repercussions at several levels: the individual must suffer the consequences of his or her actions; there may be a victim; and valuable staff time is spent adjudicating the outcome. A number of studies have suggested that an adjunct to behavioural training in institutions is a reduction in institutional misbehaviour—a suggestion which the present study sought to capitalize upon in a direct manner. In a borstal for young male law-breakers, referrals of individuals who received above average numbers of discipline reports in the institution were gathered. These people were then offered a place on a training course designed to attempt to modify behaviour towards authority figures, prison officers in particular. The course was carried out in two forms: a ‘Short’ Course of 4 days duration, and a ‘Long” Course spread over 8 weeks. Assessment of the effects of training was made by monitoring levels of discipline reports, and also by the administration of self-report and personality scales. Compared with No-training and Non-referred Controls, no significant changes in the frequency of receiving discipline reports was found following either mode of training. However, a number of changes on the self-report and personality measures suggested some degree of change after training. Implications for the construction of future courses with similar objectives are discussed. 相似文献
220.
A study was designed to tap children's perceptions of the divorce situation as opposed to adults' perceptions of the child's response to the parental divorce. Subjects were 75 boys and 75 girls from mother- and father-headed families. Using an individual structured interview procedure children were encouraged to reconstruct both positive and negative dimensions of their interactions with the custodial parent. Through factor analyses of the contents of interviews four negative and two positive factors involving 20 specific items were identified in the children's perceptual data. Subsequently, chi-square analyses were done to compare differences in girls' and boys' perceptions between and across father- and mother-headed families. Results are discussed in terms of their relationship with the existing literature and their clinical and educational relevance to children, single parents and professionals. 相似文献