全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1071篇 |
免费 | 57篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
1135篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 35篇 |
2020年 | 7篇 |
2019年 | 95篇 |
2018年 | 110篇 |
2017年 | 117篇 |
2016年 | 64篇 |
2015年 | 59篇 |
2014年 | 40篇 |
2013年 | 58篇 |
2012年 | 20篇 |
2011年 | 27篇 |
2010年 | 12篇 |
2009年 | 24篇 |
2008年 | 15篇 |
2007年 | 20篇 |
2006年 | 9篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 9篇 |
2003年 | 7篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 39篇 |
1984年 | 33篇 |
1983年 | 29篇 |
1982年 | 32篇 |
1981年 | 28篇 |
1980年 | 37篇 |
1979年 | 43篇 |
1978年 | 30篇 |
1977年 | 23篇 |
1976年 | 21篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有1135条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
191.
192.
193.
Elizabeth Urban 《The Journal of analytical psychology》2018,63(2):228-240
In this paper the author considers Descartes’ place in current thinking about the mind‐body dilemma. The premise here is that in the history of ideas, the questions posed can be as significant as the answers acquired. Descartes’ paramount question was ‘How do we determine certainty?’ and his pursuit of an answer led to cogito ergo sum. His discovery simultaneously raised the question whether mind is separate from or unified with the body. Some who currently hold that brain and subjectivity are unified contend that the philosopher ‘split’ mind from body and refer to ‘Descartes’ error’. This paper puts forward that Descartes’ detractors fail to recognise Descartes’ contribution to Western thought, which was to introduce the Enlightenment and to give a place to human subjectivity. Added to this, evidence from Descartes’ correspondence with Princess Elisabeth of Bohemia supports the conclusion that Descartes did in fact believe in the unity of mind and body although he could not reconcile this rationally with the certainty from personal experience that they were separate substances. In this Descartes was engaged in just the same dilemma as that of current thinkers and researchers, a conflict which still is yet to be resolved. 相似文献
194.
195.
Subjects were required to match the intensity levels of the left- and right-ear members of dichotically presented nonsense syllables. When subjects matched the overall average intensities of a sequence of differing dichotic pairs no ear differences were observed. When repetitions of single dichotic pairs were matched right-ear syllables were judged louder than left-ear syllables. The results support a model predicting left-hemisphere superiority in a task permitting higher-level encoding of speech input. 相似文献
196.
Erik Goodwyn 《The Journal of analytical psychology》2019,64(5):720-737
During the course of the 2018 IAAP conference, a criticism of Jung’s idea of the archetype as inherited predisposition was raised that involved examining a number of dreams and visions and assessing them through developments in genetics and neuroscience. From this comparison it was argued that archetypes cannot be inherited and could more reasonably be argued to derive from early experiences. In this essay, the author responds by showing how this conclusion is flawed due to being based on reductive errors. An alternative, non‐reductive but inherited and biological position on the archetype is defended. 相似文献
197.
198.
Joyce L. C. Ma Kelly Y. C. Lai E. S. F. Wan Lily L. L. Xia 《Journal of Family Therapy》2019,41(4):599-619
This paper reports the results of our study that assessed the treatment efficacy of multiple family therapy (MFT) from the perspective of participating Chinese children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and identified their subjective experiences. Forty‐three children with ADHD in the experimental group (EG) completed a forty‐two‐hour MFT, whilst forty‐five children with ADHD in the control group (CG) had attended two writing classes scheduled three months apart that were similar to those of the MFT. Data from the outcome study were gathered using standardized questionnaires and data from the qualitative study were drawn from thirteen children with ADHD who had completed the MFT and attended the focus group interviews (n = 5) or individual interviews (n = 8) conducted in the post‐treatment phase. The results of the Paired sample t‐test and MANOVA indicated no significant changes on the scores for the four measures adopted (perception of competence, hopefulness, parent‐child relationship and perceived social support) in the pre‐ and post‐treatment for the EG and the CG. Five themes emerged from the narratives of children with ADHD, which revealed the children’s subjective experiences with the MFT: (a) full of fun; (b) building friendships through common experiences; (c) a happy family time; (d) safe space; and (e) positive parental responses and communications. 相似文献
199.
200.
Arthur Walker‐Jones 《Zygon》2017,52(4):1005-1028
Recently the paleoanthropologist Pat Shipman has proposed what she calls the animal connection as the human trait that connects all other traits. Theologians and biblical scholars have proposed many relational, functional, and ontological interpretations of the image of God in humans and human nature, but have generally not included a connection with animals. Genesis 1–3, however, weaves human and animal creation in a variety of ways, and Adam's naming of other species implies they are understood as family or kin. Thus Genesis 1–3 understands a relationship with other animals as integral to human becoming and uses family or kinship as a root metaphor for human–animal relations. 相似文献