全文获取类型
收费全文 | 204篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
国内免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
240篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 1篇 |
2024年 | 2篇 |
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 12篇 |
2019年 | 18篇 |
2018年 | 12篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 10篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 33篇 |
2012年 | 8篇 |
2011年 | 9篇 |
2010年 | 4篇 |
2009年 | 9篇 |
2008年 | 7篇 |
2007年 | 12篇 |
2006年 | 5篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 3篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 5篇 |
2001年 | 6篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有240条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2013,66(7):1296-1305
We investigated the interaction between morphological structure and transposed-letter priming using the same–different task with Hebrew, a Semitic language in which morphology has been shown to play a key role in visual word recognition. In contrast to the results observed with lexical decision (e.g., Velan & Frost, 2009, 2011), a transposed-letter priming effect was observed irrespective of the morphological structure of the words. We take these results to suggest that morphological decomposition occurs only in the service of lexical access. We discuss further a unique feature of written Arabic, another Semitic language, to explain the apparent conflict between our findings and those reported by Perea, Abu Mallouh, García-Orza, and Carreiras (2010). 相似文献
152.
《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2013,66(11):2190-2201
In the present study two separate stimulus–response compatibility effects (functional affordance and Simon-like effects) were investigated with centrally presented pictures of an object tool (a torch) characterized by a structural separation between the graspable portion and the goal-directed portion. In Experiment 1, participants were required to decide whether the torch was red or blue, while in Experiment 2 they were required to decide whether the torch was upright or inverted. Our results showed that with the same stimulus two types of compatibility effect emerged: one based on the direction signalled by the goal-directed portion of the tool (a Simon-like effect as observed in Experiment 1), and the other based on the actions associated with an object (a functional affordance effect as observed in Experiment 2). Both effects emerged independently of the person's intention to act on the stimulus, but depended on the stimulus properties that were processed in order to perform the task. 相似文献
153.
154.
A basic function of cognition is to detect regularities in sensory input to facilitate the prediction and recognition of future events. It has been proposed that these implicit expectations arise from an internal predictive coding model, based on knowledge acquired through processes such as statistical learning, but it is unclear how different types of statistical information affect listeners’ memory for auditory stimuli. We used a combination of behavioral and computational methods to investigate memory for non‐linguistic auditory sequences. Participants repeatedly heard tone sequences varying systematically in their information‐theoretic properties. Expectedness ratings of tones were collected during three listening sessions, and a recognition memory test was given after each session. Information‐theoretic measures of sequential predictability significantly influenced listeners’ expectedness ratings, and variations in these properties had a significant impact on memory performance. Predictable sequences yielded increasingly better memory performance with increasing exposure. Computational simulations using a probabilistic model of auditory expectation suggest that listeners dynamically formed a new, and increasingly accurate, implicit cognitive model of the information‐theoretic structure of the sequences throughout the experimental session. 相似文献
155.
Daniel Williams 《Philosophical explorations》2018,21(2):245-263
I identify three lessons from Kenneth Craik’s landmark book “The Nature of Explanation” for contemporary debates surrounding the existence, extent, and nature of mental representation: first, an account of mental representations as neural structures that function analogously to public models; second, an appreciation of prediction as the central component of intelligence in demand of such models; and third, a metaphor for understanding the brain as an engineer, not a scientist. I then relate these insights to discussions surrounding the representational status of predictive processing – which, I argue, provides a contemporary vindication of Craik’s extremely prescient “hypothesis on the nature of thought.” 相似文献
156.
157.
该研究从年龄效应这一独特视角探讨了内隐序列学习的表征机制。结果表明:(1)内隐序列学习存在具体联结和抽象层级编码双重表征机制,且在该研究中以抽象编码为主;(2)老、青、幼三组被试的内隐序列学习无显著差异,间接支持了具体联结机制不起主要作用的结论。该研究的推论是:(1)内隐序列学习的表征是一个遵循“最优级编码”的动态过程;(2)该过程不涉及注意机制和短时记忆。 相似文献
158.
Building to discover: a common coding model 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Chandrasekharan S 《Cognitive Science》2009,33(6):1059-1086
I present a case study of scientific discovery, where building two functional and behavioral approximations of neurons, one physical and the other computational, led to conceptual and implementation breakthroughs in a neural engineering laboratory. Such building of external systems that mimic target phenomena, and the use of these external systems to generate novel concepts and control structures, is a standard strategy in the new engineering sciences. I develop a model of the cognitive mechanism that connects such built external systems with internal models, and I examine how new discoveries, and consensus on discoveries, could arise from this external-internal coupling and the building process. The model is based on the emerging framework of common coding, which proposes a shared representation in the brain between the execution, perception, and imagination of movement. 相似文献
159.
Aarno Laitila Jukka Aaltonen Jarl Wahlström Lynne Angus 《Contemporary Family Therapy》2001,23(3):309-322
The aim of this study is to investigate whether the method (Narrative Processes Coding System) for studying the narrative sequences of individual psychotherapy developed by Angus and Hardtke (1994) can be applied in a family therapeutic process. According to the results narrative process coding system adds new dimensions and gives new depth in the narrative understanding of family therapeutic process. In micro-analytic interactional level it shows the complicated interactions between narration, plot, and narrative process type. 相似文献
160.
Wolfram Boucsein Florian Schaefer Evgeni N. Sokolov Christina Schröder John J. Furedy 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》2001,36(2):137-153
A framework for accounting for emotional phenomena proposed by Sokolov and Boucsein (2000) employs conceptual dimensions that
parallel those of hue, brightness, and saturation in color vision. The approach that employs the concepts of emotional quality,
intensity, and saturation has been supported by psychophysical emotional scaling data gathered from a few trained observers.
We report cortical evoked potential data obtained during the change between different emotions expressed in schematic faces.
Twenty-five subjects (13 male, 12 female) were presented with a positive, a negative, and a neutral computer-generated face
with random interstimulus intervals in a within-subjects design, together with four meaningful and four meaningless control
stimuli made up from the same elements. Frontal, central, parietal, and temporal ERPs were recorded from each hemisphere.
Statistically significant outcomes in the P300 and N200 range support the potential fruitfulness of the proposed color-vision-model-based
approach to human emotional space. 相似文献