全文获取类型
收费全文 | 804篇 |
免费 | 56篇 |
国内免费 | 24篇 |
专业分类
884篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 5篇 |
2024年 | 25篇 |
2023年 | 36篇 |
2022年 | 34篇 |
2021年 | 36篇 |
2020年 | 52篇 |
2019年 | 52篇 |
2018年 | 40篇 |
2017年 | 49篇 |
2016年 | 47篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 24篇 |
2013年 | 73篇 |
2012年 | 13篇 |
2011年 | 13篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 17篇 |
2008年 | 17篇 |
2007年 | 19篇 |
2006年 | 23篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 18篇 |
2003年 | 12篇 |
2002年 | 20篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 8篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 16篇 |
1978年 | 9篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有884条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
181.
Recently, a number of epistemologists have argued that there are no non-conceptual elements in representational content. On their view, the only sort of non-conceptual elements are components of sub-personal organic hardware that, because they enjoy no veridical role, must be construed epistemologically irrelevant. By reviewing a 35-year-old debate initiated by Dagfinn Følledal, I believe Husserlian phenomenology can be updated to offer an important contribution to this discussion. On my interpretation, what Husserl calls “hyletic data” may be read as that subjective quality of experience inarticulable as a propositional attitude – and, thus, hyletic data are non-conceptual. In anticipation of the recent conceptualist position, Føllesdal and his adherents argued that what Husserl had called “noema” or representational content is, however, entirely conceptual. A closer inspection of the relevant texts, however, reveals that Husserl admits non-conceptual elements into his characterization of the noema. If that is correct, then Husserl must have been a dualist about non-conceptual content. In turn, I believe what explains this dualism is a non-foundationalist reconstruction of Husserl’s phenomenological reduction.An earlier version of this paper was delivered at Fordham University on the occasion of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Husserl Circle. I am grateful to Prof. Steven Crowell who, in his capacity as respondent, offered incisive and helpful commentary. I also thank Prof. John J. Drummond for organizing the meeting, and all those present during my talk; as well as two anonymous reviewers for Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, who offered helpful questions and comments. 相似文献
182.
Nilam?RamEmail author Sy-Miin?Chow Ryan?P.?Bowles Lijuan?Wang Kevin?Grimm Frank?Fujita John?R.?Nesselroade 《Psychometrika》2005,70(4):773-790
Weekly cycles in emotion were examined by combining item response modeling and spectral analysis approaches in an analysis
of 179 college students' reports of daily emotions experienced over 7 weeks. We addressed the measurement of emotion using
an item response model. Spectral analysis and multilevel sinusoidal models were used to identify interindividual differences
in intraindividual cyclic change. Simulations and incomplete data designs were used to examine how well this combination of
analysis techniques might work when applied to other practical data problems. Empirically, we found systematic individual
differences in the extent to which individuals' emotions follow a weekly cycle, and in how such cycles are exhibited. Weekly
cycles accounted for very little variance in day to day emotions at the individual level. Analytically, we illustrate how
measurement, change, and interindividual difference models from different traditions may be combined in a practical manner
to describe some of the complexities of human behavior.
The authors gratefully acknowledge the support provided by grant T32 AG20500 from the National Institute on Aging in the preparation
of this article. Special thanks to those at the Institute for Developmental and Health Research Methodology at the University
of Virginia and to Paul De Boeck and the reviewers for helpful comments on earlier versions of this work. 相似文献
183.
Spontaneous number discrimination of multi-format auditory stimuli in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Studies using operant training have demonstrated that laboratory animals can discriminate the number of objects or events based on either auditory or visual stimuli, as well as the integration of both auditory and visual modalities. To date, studies of spontaneous number discrimination in untrained animals have been restricted to the visual modality, leaving open the question of whether such capacities generalize to other modalities such as audition. To explore the capacity to spontaneously discriminate number based on auditory stimuli, and to assess the abstractness of the representation underlying this capacity, a habituation-discrimination procedure involving speech and pure tones was used with a colony of cotton-top tamarins. In the habituation phase, we presented subjects with either two- or three-speech syllable sequences that varied with respect to overall duration, inter-syllable duration, and pitch. In the test phase, we presented subjects with a counterbalanced order of either two- or three-tone sequences that also varied with respect to overall duration, inter-syllable duration, and pitch. The proportion of looking responses to test stimuli differing in number was significantly greater than to test stimuli consisting of the same number. Combined with earlier work, these results show that at least one non-human primate species can spontaneously discriminate number in both the visual and auditory domain, indicating that this capacity is not tied to a particular modality, and within a modality, can accommodate differences in format. 相似文献
184.
EM and beyond 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Donald B. Rubin 《Psychometrika》1991,56(2):241-254
The basic theme of the EM algorithm, to repeatedly use complete-data methods to solve incomplete data problems, is also a theme of several more recent statistical techniques. These techniques—multiple imputation, data augmentation, stochastic relaxation, and sampling importance resampling—combine simulation techniques with complete-data methods to attack problems that are difficult or impossible for EM.A preliminary version of this article was the Keynote Address at the 1987 European Meeting of the Psychometric Society June 24–26, 1987 in Enschede, The Netherlands. The author wishes to thank the editor and reviewers for helpful comments. 相似文献
185.
《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2013,66(9):1721-1739
We examine the influence of contrast categories on the internal graded membership structure of everyday concepts using computational models proposed in the artificial category learning tradition. In particular, the generalized context model (Nosofsky, 1986), which assumes that only members of a given category contribute to the typicality of a category member, is contrasted to the similarity–dissimilarity generalized context model (SD-GCM; Stewart & Brown, 2005), which assumes that members of other categories are also influential in determining typicality. The models are compared in a hierarchical Bayesian framework in their account of the typicality gradient of five animal categories and six artefact categories. For each target category, we consider all possible relevant contrast categories. Three separate issue are examined: (a) whether contrast effects can be found, (b) which categories are responsible for these effects, and (c) whether more than one category influences the typicality. Results indicate that the internal category structure is codetermined by dissimilarity towards potential contrast categories. In most cases, only a single contrast category contributed to the typicality. The present findings suggest that contrast effects might be more widespread than has previously been assumed. Further, they stress the importance of characteristics particular of everyday concepts, which require careful consideration when applying computational models of representation of the artificial category learning tradition to everyday concepts. 相似文献
186.
187.
Sy-Miin Chow Jason J. Bendezú Pamela M. Cole Nilam Ram 《Multivariate behavioral research》2016,51(2-3):154-184
Several approaches exist for estimating the derivatives of observed data for model exploration purposes, including functional data analysis (FDA; Ramsay &; Silverman, 2005), generalized local linear approximation (GLLA; Boker, Deboeck, Edler, &; Peel, 2010), and generalized orthogonal local derivative approximation (GOLD; Deboeck, 2010). These derivative estimation procedures can be used in a two-stage process to fit mixed effects ordinary differential equation (ODE) models. While the performance and utility of these routines for estimating linear ODEs have been established, they have not yet been evaluated in the context of nonlinear ODEs with mixed effects. We compared properties of the GLLA and GOLD to an FDA-based two-stage approach denoted herein as functional ordinary differential equation with mixed effects (FODEmixed) in a Monte Carlo (MC) study using a nonlinear coupled oscillators model with mixed effects. Simulation results showed that overall, the FODEmixed outperformed both the GLLA and GOLD across all the embedding dimensions considered, but a novel use of a fourth-order GLLA approach combined with very high embedding dimensions yielded estimation results that almost paralleled those from the FODEmixed. We discuss the strengths and limitations of each approach and demonstrate how output from each stage of FODEmixed may be used to inform empirical modeling of young children’s self-regulation. 相似文献
188.
Emilio Ferrer Rand D. Conger Richard W. Robins 《Multivariate behavioral research》2016,51(2-3):279-295
We examine the dynamics of substance use and psychiatric symptoms from childhood to adolescence using a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin families (N = 674). We present a longitudinal model capturing the trajectories of substance use and psychiatric symptoms (depression, conduct disorder), as well as the interrelations between these trajectories over time. Such a model is an extension of latent change score models designed to account for the occurrence of psychiatric symptoms while also accommodating a large amount of zeros for nonoccurrence and characterizing the changes over time in the count data. We compare this model with a more traditional approach based on a log transformation of the data. We describe differences between these approaches and highlight the benefits of using the two-part model when the data include a large amount of zeros for nonoccurrence of the behavior. 相似文献
189.
Anna Brown 《Multivariate behavioral research》2016,51(2-3):345-356
To prevent response bias, personality questionnaires may use comparative response formats. These include forced choice, where respondents choose among a number of items, and quantitative comparisons, where respondents indicate the extent to which items are preferred to each other. The present article extends Thurstonian modeling of binary choice data to “proportion-of-total” (compositional) formats. Following the seminal work of Aitchison, compositional item data are transformed into log ratios, conceptualized as differences of latent item utilities. The mean and covariance structure of the log ratios is modeled using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), where the item utilities are first-order factors, and personal attributes measured by a questionnaire are second-order factors. A simulation study with two sample sizes, N = 300 and N = 1,000, shows that the method provides very good recovery of true parameters and near-nominal rejection rates. The approach is illustrated with empirical data from N = 317 students, comparing model parameters obtained with compositional and Likert-scale versions of a Big Five measure. The results show that the proposed model successfully captures the latent structures and person scores on the measured traits. 相似文献
190.