全文获取类型
收费全文 | 627篇 |
免费 | 103篇 |
国内免费 | 34篇 |
专业分类
764篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 2篇 |
2024年 | 16篇 |
2023年 | 19篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 25篇 |
2020年 | 31篇 |
2019年 | 39篇 |
2018年 | 28篇 |
2017年 | 31篇 |
2016年 | 15篇 |
2015年 | 20篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 91篇 |
2012年 | 11篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 21篇 |
2009年 | 26篇 |
2008年 | 25篇 |
2007年 | 27篇 |
2006年 | 29篇 |
2005年 | 32篇 |
2004年 | 20篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 28篇 |
2001年 | 31篇 |
2000年 | 16篇 |
1999年 | 11篇 |
1998年 | 14篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有764条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
Three adult male sex offenders with developmental disabilities participated in an evaluation of presession factors that may influence levels of sexual arousal measured with a penile plethysmograph. We evaluated the effects of presession masturbation (1 participant) and arousal-suppression strategies (2 participants). Results showed that presession masturbation lowered arousal levels and both participants suppressed arousal to varying degrees. These outcomes suggest the potential for consideration and manipulation of presession factors as treatment components for sex offenders with developmental disabilities. 相似文献
72.
Almost all admit that there is beauty in the natural world. Many suspect that such beauty is more than an adornment of nature. Few in our contemporary world suggest that this beauty is an empirical principle of the natural world itself and instead relegate beauty to the eye and mind of the beholder. Guided by theological and scientific insight, the authors propose that such exclusion is no longer tenable, at least in the data of modern biology and in our view of the natural world in general. More important, we believe an empirical aesthetics exists that can help guide experimental design and development of computational models in biology. Moreover, because theology and science can both contribute toward and equally profit from such an aesthetics, we propose that this empirical aesthetics provides the foundation for a living synergy between theology and science. 相似文献
73.
Emily McKendry‐Smith 《Journal for the scientific study of religion》2016,55(4):698-716
This article extends and complicates the theory that “strict churches are strong” based on the strict demands that were followed and not followed by the Brahma Kumaris in Nepal. To be successful, strict religious groups must not only be strict, but also frame their strictness in ways that resonate with the everyday experiences and commonly held beliefs of their members. Nepali women involved with the Brahma Kumaris tend to accept and follow the group's demands when those demands have been framed as modern, and that framing is resonant with the prevailing definitions of modernity offered by Western development agencies. When the modern framing is not resonant, as with the case of framing celibacy as “spiritual birth control,” the strict demand is not followed, and the frame itself is rejected in favor of the practice being defined in different terms. 相似文献
74.
The purpose of this study was to investigate cultural differences between Danish and American children at 2 and 3 years as measured with the developmental test Bayley‐III, and to investigate the Bayley‐III Language Scale validity. The Danish children (N = 43) were tested with the Bayley‐III and their parents completed an additional language questionnaire (the MacArthur‐Bates CDI). Results showed that scores from the Danish children did not differ significantly from the American norms on the Cognitive or Motor Scale, but the Danish sample scored significantly higher on the Language Scale. A comparison of the Bayley‐III Language subtests with the CDI showed that the two measures correlated significantly, but the percentile score from the CDI was significantly higher than the percentile score from the Bayley‐III Language subtests. This could be because the two instruments measure slightly different areas of language development, or because the Bayley‐III overestimates language development in Danish children. However, due to the limitations of the current study, further research is needed to clarify this issue. 相似文献
75.
Michele A. Craft Sheila R. Alber William L. Heward 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1998,31(3):399-415
Four fourth graders with developmental disabilities were trained to recruit teacher attention while they worked on spelling assignments in a general education classroom. The students were taught to show their work to the teacher two to three times per session and to make statements such as, “How am I doing?” or “Look, I'm all finished!” Training was conducted in the special education classroom and consisted of modeling, role playing, error correction, and praise. A multiple baseline across students design showed that recruitment training increased (a) the frequency of students' recruiting, (b) the frequency of teacher praise received by the students, (c) the percentage of worksheet items completed, and (d) the accuracy with which the students completed the spelling assignments. 相似文献
76.
Most concepts of development explain certain behavior changes as products or markers of the invariable succession of emerging periods, stages, refinements, or achievements that define and order much of an individual's life. A different but comparable concept can be derived from the most basic mechanisms of behavior analysis, which are its environmental contingencies, and from its most basic strategy, which is to study behavior as its subject matter. From a behavior-analytic perspective, the most fundamental developmental questions are (a) whether these contingencies vary in any systematic way across the life span, and thus make behavior change in a correspondingly systematic way; and (b) whether some of these contingencies and their changes have more far-reaching consequences than others, in terms of the importance to the organism and others, of the behavior classes they change. Certain behavior changes open the door to especially broad or especially important further behavior change, leading to the concept of the behavioral cusp. A behavioral cusp, then, is any behavior change that brings the organism's behavior into contact with new contingencies that have even more far-reaching consequences. Of all the environmental contingencies that change or maintain behavior, those that accomplish cusps are developmental. Behavior change remains the fundamental phenomenon of development for a behavior-analytic view; a cusp is a special instance of behavior change, a change crucial to what can come next. 相似文献
77.
Meeta R. Patel Cathleen C. Piazza Markell L. Kelly Corrine A. Ochsner Christine M. Santana 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2001,34(3):357-360
Stimulus fading was combined with differential reinforcement and extinction to increase intake of a calorie‐dense fluid by a 6‐year‐old child with feeding problems. The fading procedure consisted of adding Carnation Instant Breakfast? Final acceptance and then milk to water (a fluid the child would drink). 相似文献
78.
Although experimental effects typically are evaluated by summarizing levels of responding across time (e.g., calculating the mean levels of problem behavior during 10‐min sessions), these data summaries may obscure important mechanisms that may be responsible for changes in responding. A case study is reported to illustrate alternative methods of data analysis when decreasing trends in responding may be due to increases in response efficiency. 相似文献
79.
Janean E. Dilworth‐Bart Ayesha Khurshid Deborah Lowe Vandell 《Infant and child development》2007,16(5):525-552
Using Ecological Systems Theory and stage sequential modelling procedures for detecting mediation, this study examined how early developmental contexts impact preschoolers' performances on a measure of sustained attention and impulse control. Data from 1273 European‐American and African‐American participants in the NICHD Study of Early Child Care were used to identify the potential mediators of the relation between early household income‐to‐need (INR) and 54‐month impulsivity and inattention. Exploratory analyses were also conducted to determine whether the relationships between early income, home environment, parenting stress, and the outcome variables differ for African‐American versus European‐American‐American children. We found modest support for the study hypothesis that 36‐month home environment quality mediated the INR/attention relationship. INR accounted for more home environment score variance and home environment accounted for more Impulsivity score variance for African‐American children. Home environments were related to inattention in the European‐American, but not African‐American, group. Copyright © 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
80.
Lindauer SE Zarcone JR Richman DM Schroeder SR 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2002,35(3):299-303
Functional analysis results indicated that maladaptive behaviors displayed by a 25-year-old man with profound mental retardation were maintained by attention from caregivers and possibly, to a lesser degree, by access to tangible items. A concurrent-schedules procedure was then used to study the relative reinforcing value for maladaptive behavior of attention versus tangible items. Results of the concurrent-schedules assessment and subsequent functional communication training indicated that preference for attention versus access to a tangible object varied. 相似文献