首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   977篇
  免费   114篇
  国内免费   93篇
  2024年   3篇
  2023年   32篇
  2022年   15篇
  2021年   32篇
  2020年   41篇
  2019年   63篇
  2018年   53篇
  2017年   59篇
  2016年   39篇
  2015年   28篇
  2014年   44篇
  2013年   139篇
  2012年   35篇
  2011年   32篇
  2010年   26篇
  2009年   43篇
  2008年   43篇
  2007年   55篇
  2006年   54篇
  2005年   35篇
  2004年   36篇
  2003年   32篇
  2002年   46篇
  2001年   24篇
  2000年   23篇
  1999年   18篇
  1998年   18篇
  1997年   7篇
  1996年   12篇
  1995年   8篇
  1994年   14篇
  1993年   10篇
  1992年   8篇
  1991年   7篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   6篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   1篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   5篇
  1978年   6篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   1篇
排序方式: 共有1184条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
在计算机自适应性测验(CAT)中,传统的项目选题策略正面对越来越多的问题,比如:测验的安全,项目的曝光率,项目的平衡应用等等。分层选题策略的新发展-A-STR和BAS-选题策略部分地解决了传统的选题策略所面临的难题。能够有效地控制高区分度项目的曝光率,增加低区分度项目的曝光率,平衡项目的应用,提高测验效率,降低测验成本等等。为计算机自适应测验的选题策略提供了一种更加有效的方法,也为我国开展计算机自适应测验提供了一种思路。  相似文献   
992.
视觉选择性注意脑机制研究进展   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
视觉选择性注意一直是认知心理学的研究热点之一。注意的早晚期选择问题和基于空间与基于客体的选择问题是选择性注意研究中长期争论的两大理论问题。脑成像研究不仅为解决这些争论提供了有力的证据 ,而且深化和拓展了这两大问题的研究。该文介绍了脑成像研究在上述两个领域的主要研究成果 ,并展望了今后相关问题的研究重点。  相似文献   
993.
斯蒂芬·杰伊·古尔德和理查德·道金斯是著名的进化生物学家和颇有影响的媒体科学家,他们在进化论的很多方面都有不同意见."进化是进步的吗?"是古尔德和道金斯之争的核心问题.在追溯这一问题的历史的基础之上,考察古尔德和道金斯观点的分歧以及探讨这种分歧背后的可能原因.  相似文献   
994.
This study describes the developmental course of the social context of feeding behavior in infants and yearlings of Japanese macaques in a provisioned free-ranging troop. Results of this longitudinal study revealed that infants and yearlings exhibited synchronous feeding behavior in the majority of cases in which they had feeding neighbors within 1 m. They consistently showed feeding behavior more often when their closest neighbor fed than when they did not. In feeding situations, infants tended mainly to be in close proximity with their mothers in the early stages of life and, later, also with peers. The results imply that potential opportunities for the social transmission of food selection habits are available for young Japanese macaques in the wild.  相似文献   
995.
In a 3-year longitudinal study, we found in a sample of young professionals (N=82; 44% male; age range: 28 to 39 years) that self-reported behaviors reflecting selection, optimization, and compensation (SOC) predicted global and work-specific subjective well-being (multiple correlations ranged from R=.22 to R=.44). In addition to optimization (i.e., implementing goal-relevant means), it was especially the degree of compensation (i.e., investing goal-relevant means to counteract losses) that predicted how emotionally balanced individuals felt and how satisfied they were with their work situation 3 years later. These longitudinal predictions were quite robust when controlling for personality variables (NEO). Results are consistent with previous cross-sectional findings and demonstrate how the SOC framework might be successfully applied to the domain of vocational behavior.  相似文献   
996.
In numerous languages determiner forms depend not only on semantic information but also on several other kinds of information, such as the grammatical gender of the controlling noun or the phonological properties of the context. In the present research we contrasted two possible accounts of determiner retrieval: one in which every type of required information is bundled into a unitized representation for determiner retrieval and one in which each type of information can individually activate determiner forms. These alternative hypotheses were investigated in three experiments in which native speakers of French named pictures with simple [determiner + noun] or complex [determiner + adjective + noun] noun phrases. In the experiments, the properties of the contextual cues that drive the retrieval of the determiner were manipulated - for example, we manipulated the number of determiner forms that are compatible with a given grammatical gender and the number of grammatical genders that a given determiner form can be used with. Neither hypothesis can fully account for the results of the three experiments. However, a hybrid hypothesis that combines the principal features of the two hypotheses provides a good account of the data.  相似文献   
997.
Michael Ruse 《Zygon》2002,37(3):581-596
The seventeenth–century chemist and philosopher Robert Boyle argued that the world is like a clockwork machine. This led to the problems of the place of a Creator and of how one can explain the directed, "final–cause" nature of organisms. Boyle thought that he could wrap everything up in one neat package, with a clear place for a designing God, but of course the coming of Darwinism casts doubt on this. Nevertheless, Boyle's thinking does have some very interesting implications for the way in which we today should consider the science/religion relationship.  相似文献   
998.
Review     
《Zygon》2002,37(3):763-766
  相似文献   
999.
Kerry Gordon 《Zygon》2002,37(4):963-983
Beginning with relativity and quantum theory, the deterministic view that has dominated and shaped Western culture for more than 2,500 years has begun to unravel, leading to the emergence of a new paradigm. This new paradigm effectively reformulates the project of science, conceiving of existence as an interpenetrating web of coevolving, cocreative relationships. By exploring Kabbalah and the new scientific paradigm within the context of shared evolutionary principles, I seek to demonstrate a viable alternative to the prevailing deterministic worldview. By going beyond the limits of determinism and re–visioning existence as an evolutionary, emergent phenomenon, we can establish a new basis for an authentic dialogue between science and religion.  相似文献   
1000.
When someone is asked to speak his mind, it is sometimes possible for him to furnish what his utterance appears to have omitted. In such cases we might say that he had a mind to speak. Sometimes, however, the opposite is true. Asked to speak his mind, our speaker finds that he has no mind to speak. When it is possible to speak one's mind and when not is largely determined by the kinds of beings we are and by the kinds of resources we are able to draw upon. In either case, not speaking one's mind is leaving something out whose articulation would or could matter for the purposes for which one was speaking in the first place. Inarticulation is no fleetingly contingent and peripheral phenomenon in human thinking and discourse. It is a substantial and dominant commonplace. In Part One I attempt to say something about what it is about the human agent that makes inarticulateness so rife. In Part Two, I consider various strategies for making the unarticulated explicit, and certain constraints on such processes. I shall suggest, among other things, that standard treatments of enthymematic reconstruction are fundamentally misconceived.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号