全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4024篇 |
免费 | 650篇 |
国内免费 | 201篇 |
专业分类
4875篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 19篇 |
2024年 | 82篇 |
2023年 | 148篇 |
2022年 | 138篇 |
2021年 | 173篇 |
2020年 | 241篇 |
2019年 | 330篇 |
2018年 | 282篇 |
2017年 | 261篇 |
2016年 | 254篇 |
2015年 | 159篇 |
2014年 | 228篇 |
2013年 | 642篇 |
2012年 | 160篇 |
2011年 | 146篇 |
2010年 | 151篇 |
2009年 | 128篇 |
2008年 | 142篇 |
2007年 | 147篇 |
2006年 | 139篇 |
2005年 | 145篇 |
2004年 | 100篇 |
2003年 | 98篇 |
2002年 | 103篇 |
2001年 | 62篇 |
2000年 | 61篇 |
1999年 | 35篇 |
1998年 | 44篇 |
1997年 | 30篇 |
1996年 | 29篇 |
1995年 | 38篇 |
1994年 | 31篇 |
1993年 | 21篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有4875条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
301.
Facial information and attention to facial displays are distributed over spatial as well as temporal domains. Thus far, research on selective attention to (dis)approving faces in the context of social anxiety has concentrated primarily on the spatial domain. Using a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) paradigm, the present study examined the temporal characteristics of visual attention for happy and angry faces in high- (n=16) and low-socially anxious individuals (n=17), to test whether also in the temporal domain socially anxious individuals are characterized by threat-confirming attentional biases. Results indicated that presenting angry faces as the first target (T1) did not aggravate the detection of the emotional expression of the second target (T2). Yet, participants generally showed superior detection of the emotional expression of T2, if T2 was an angry face. Casting doubt on the role of such attenuated attentional blink for angry faces in social anxiety, no evidence emerged to indicate that this effect was relatively strong in high-socially anxious individuals. Finally, the presentation of an angry face as T2 resulted in a relatively hampered identification of a happy-T1. Again, this "backward blink" was not especially pronounced in high-socially anxious individuals. The present anger superiority effects are consistent with evolutionary models stressing the importance of being especially vigilant for signals of dominance. Since the effects were not especially pronounced in high-anxious individuals, the present study adds to previous findings indicating that socially anxious individuals are not characterized by a bias in the (explicit) detection of emotional expressions [Philippot, P., & Douilliez, C. (2005). Social phobics do not misinterpret facial expression of emotion. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 639-652]. 相似文献
302.
采用负启动研究范式,考察九岁儿童在类皮亚杰数量守恒任务中的负启动效应。实验一重复了Houdé等人的实验过程,发现我国儿童在解决类皮亚杰数量守恒任务中也存在负启动效应,负启动量为310.9ms;实验二改进了Houdé等人研究的实验材料,排除实验材料形状的干扰,发现依然存在负启动效应,负启动量为200.04ms。实验结果表明,成功完成类皮亚杰数量守恒任务需要认知抑制过程的参与。 相似文献
303.
Use of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in the Early Identification of Alzheimer's Disease 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
A growing body of evidence suggests that a preclinical phase of Alzheimer's disease (AD) exists several years or more prior
to the overt manifestation of clinical symptoms and is characterized by subtle neuropsychological and brain changes. Identification
of individuals prior to the development of significant clinical symptoms is imperative in order to have the greatest treatment
impact by maintaining cognitive abilities and preserving quality of life. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) offers
considerable promise as a non-invasive tool for detecting early functional brain changes in asymptomatic adults. In fact,
evidence to date indicates that functional brain decline precedes structural decline in preclinical samples. Therefore, fMRI
may offer the unique ability to capture the dynamic state of change in the degenerating brain. This review examines the clinical
utility of blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) fMRI in those at risk for AD as well as in early AD. We provide an overview
of fMRI findings in at-risk groups by virtue of genetic susceptibility or mild cognitive decline followed by an appraisal
of the methodological issues concerning the diagnostic usefulness of fMRI in early AD. We conclude with a discussion of future
directions and propose that BOLD-fMRI in combination with cerebral blood flow or diffusion techniques will provide a more
complete accounting of the neurovascular changes that occur in preclinical AD and thus improve our ability to reliably detect
early brain changes prior to disease onset. 相似文献
304.
There is increasing evidence to support the biological basis of mental disorders. Subsequently, understanding the neurobiological context from which mental distress arises can help counselors appropriately apply cognitive behavioral therapy and other well‐researched cognitive interventions. The purpose of this article is to describe the neurobiological context underlying the formation and treatment of posttraumatic stress disorders, a mental disorder frequently encountered by counselors, from a cognitive therapy framework. 相似文献
305.
《The Journal of social psychology》2012,152(4):351-368
ABSTRACTWe tested a mediation model of weight bias that considers person attributes and contact experiences with overweight individuals. In Study 1, we administered a survey to assess Openness, Agreeableness, Attributional Complexity, contact experiences with overweight individuals, and weight bias. Mediation analyses found that Agreeableness predicted less weight bias through contact experiences. In Study 2, we asked participants to interact with a peer whose weight and attributions regarding the weight were experimentally manipulated. We then measured acceptance of the peer. Agreeableness was found to indirectly predict more acceptance of an overweight peer through Empathy and contact experiences. These results show that contact theory is applicable to the domain of weight bias, and support person-situation approaches to prejudice. 相似文献
306.
The study tested the efficacy and tolerability of cognitive processing therapy (CPT) for survivors of assault with acute stress disorder. Participants (N = 30) were randomly allocated to CPT or supportive counseling. Therapy comprised six individual weekly sessions of 90-min duration. Independent diagnostic assessment for PTSD was conducted at posttreatment. Participants completed self-report measures of posttraumatic stress, depression, and negative trauma-related beliefs at pre-, posttreatment, and 6-month follow-up. Results indicated that both interventions were successful in reducing symptoms at posttreatment with no statistical difference between the two; within and between-group effect sizes and the proportion of participants not meeting PTSD criteria was greater in CPT. Treatment gains were maintained for both groups at 6-month follow-up. 相似文献
307.
Mirolli M 《Cognitive Science》2012,36(5):870-895
Understanding the role of 'representations' in cognitive science is a fundamental problem facing the emerging framework of embodied, situated, dynamical cognition. To make progress, I follow the approach proposed by an influential representational skeptic, Randall Beer: building artificial agents capable of minimally cognitive behaviors and assessing whether their internal states can be considered to involve representations. Hence, I operationalize the concept of representing as 'standing in,' and I look for representations in embodied agents involved in simple categorization tasks. In a first experiment, no representation can be found, but the relevance of the task is undermined by the fact that agents with no internal states can reach high performance. A simple modification makes the task more "representationally hungry," and in this case, agents' internal states are found to qualify as representations. I conclude by discussing the benefits of reconciling the embodied-dynamical approach with the notion of representation. 相似文献
308.
Young children are slower in naming the color of a meaningful picture than in naming the color of an abstract form (Stroop-like color-object interference). The current experiments tested an executive control account of this phenomenon. First, color-object interference was observed in 6- and 8-year-olds but not in 12- and 16-year-olds (Experiment 1). Second, meaningful pictures did not interfere in 5- to 7-year-olds’ manual sorting of objects on the basis of color (Experiment 2) or in their naming the number of colored objects in the display, that is, subitizing (Experiment 3). These findings provide support for the view that color-object interference results from the children’s immature inhibition of the prepotent but irrelevant task of object naming. 相似文献
309.
Timothy Z. Keith Matthew R. ReynoldsLisa G. Roberts Amanda L. WinterCynthia A. Austin 《Intelligence》2011,39(5):389
Sex differences in the latent general and broad cognitive abilities underlying the Differential Ability Scales, Second Edition were investigated for children and youth ages 5 through 17. Multi-group mean and covariance structural equation modeling was used to investigate sex differences in latent cognitive abilities as well as changes in these differences across age. Most broad abilities showed mean differences across the sexes, and all such differences varied across ages. Girls showed an advantage on the processing speed (Gs) first-order residual factor. Girls also showed advantages at some ages on free-recall memory, a narrow ability of long term retrieval (Glr). Boys showed a developmentally-related advantage on a visual-spatial ability (Gv) first-order residual factor, depending on age. Younger girls showed an advantage on short-term memory (Gsm). No statistically significant sex differences were shown on the latent comprehension-knowledge (Gc) factor, or on a second-order, latent g factor. Boys showed larger variances for several broad abilities, some substantial, but those differences were not statistically significant. 相似文献
310.