全文获取类型
收费全文 | 750篇 |
免费 | 131篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
883篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 26篇 |
2019年 | 29篇 |
2018年 | 30篇 |
2017年 | 27篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 21篇 |
2014年 | 32篇 |
2013年 | 92篇 |
2012年 | 23篇 |
2011年 | 13篇 |
2010年 | 20篇 |
2009年 | 29篇 |
2008年 | 49篇 |
2007年 | 36篇 |
2006年 | 52篇 |
2005年 | 46篇 |
2004年 | 40篇 |
2003年 | 33篇 |
2002年 | 25篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 24篇 |
1999年 | 18篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 14篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有883条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
811.
Three experiments using multiple schedules of reinforcement explored the implications of resistance-to-change findings for the response-reinforcer relation described by the law of effect, using both steady-state responding and responding recorded in the first few sessions of conditions. In Experiment 1, when response-independent reinforcement was increased during a third component, response rate in Components 1 and 2 decreased. This response-rate reduction was proportionately greater in a component in which reinforcer magnitude was small (2-s access to wheat) than in the component in which it was large (6-s access to wheat). However, when reinforcer rates in the two components were varied together in Experiments 2 and 3, response-rate change was the same regardless of the magnitude of reinforcers used in the two components, so that sensitivity of response rates to reinforcer rates (Experiment 2) and of response-rate ratios to reinforcer-rate ratios (Experiment 3) was unaffected by the magnitude of the reinforcers. Therefore, the principles determining resistance to change, described by behavioral momentum theory, seem not to apply when the source of behavior change is the variation of reinforcement contingencies that maintain the behavior. The use of extinction as a manipulation to study resistance to change is questioned. 相似文献
812.
Key pecking in pigeons was examined under concurrent and parallel arrangements of two independent and simultaneously available variable-interval schedules. Pecks on the changeover key alternated the schedule of reinforcement for responses on the main key. Under concurrent schedules, discriminative stimuli were paired with the reinforcement schedule arranged in each component and changeover responses also alternated these stimuli. Under parallel schedules, changeover responses alternated the effective reinforcement schedule, but did not change the discriminative stimulus. On concurrent procedures, changeover response rate was inversely related to the difference in reinforcement rate between the two components, whereas on parallel schedules no consistent relationship was found. With both schedules, absolute response and reinforcement rates were positively related, although for a given set of reinforcement frequencies, rates were often higher on the concurrent schedules. On concurrent schedules, relative response rates and relative times were equal to relative reinforcement rates. On parallel schedules these ratios were positively related, but response and time ratios were much smaller than were obtained with comparable concurrent schedules. This inequality was most pronounced when absolute reinforcement frequencies were lowest. 相似文献
813.
"时"观念是理解中国古代思想的重要线索。在一定层面上讲,"时"观念的延展及其实践承载着传统农业社会的绵延。"时"在《管子》中是一个基础性的核心概念,贯通全文,它传承"时"的原发观念和本质特性,融入气、阴阳、五行思想,与道、德、法相通融,"时"作为论理基准,在"治国安邦"、"经世致用"中具有统领性意义。 相似文献
814.
人类行为的刺激-心理活动-反应的数学公式 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
在一些简单情况下 ,人可由对刺激的主观体验和简单的心理加工 ,作出决定并产生反应。在假设简单心理加工的幂定律后 ,可以用一组数学公式 ,即文中的式 ( 1 ) ,( 2 ) ,( 3 ) ,( 4) ,来定量描述人类行为的刺激—心理活动—反应关系。 相似文献
815.
816.
借鉴韩非法治观构建和谐医患关系 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
近年来医院暴力愈演愈烈,医疗索赔越来越高,防御性医疗越来越明显,如何建立和谐的医患关系已成为一个亟待解决的社会问题。阐述韩非以法为治,定分止争,重刑止奸,法不阿贵和布之于百姓在医事立法与构建和谐医患关系中的借鉴作用,探讨如何通过建立法律化、规范化医患关系来缓解医患冲突,改善医患关系。 相似文献
817.
ANNEGRET KÄMPF BERNADETTE McSHERRY STUART THOMAS HELEN ABRAHAMS 《Australian psychologist》2008,43(3):194-204
This paper outlines the legal and ethical duties of psychologists in relation to preserving as well as breaching confidentiality in therapeutic relationships. It analyses the results of a questionnaire examining psychologists’ perceptions of the legal and ethical constraints on confidentiality and their likelihood of breaching confidentiality in different situations. The vast majority of participants indicated that the law permits them to disclose confidential information and that there is an ethical duty to disclose information to a third party when the patient is perceived to be dangerous. The results suggest that there is some uncertainty as to when confidentiality should be breached in practice and it is argued that the law is overly complex in this area and that guidelines are needed to assist psychologists in their clinical practice. 相似文献
818.
Forensic practice in Australia and around the world attracts a high level of public and judicial scrutiny. The way in which the forensic psychologist conducts him or herself in ethically challenging situations is important not only to the reputation of the individual practitioner, but to the profession more widely. This paper outlines some of the ethical issues that commonly arise in forensic psychology practice and discusses these in relation to the recently published Australian Psychological Society (2007) Code of Ethics. Four ethically challenging scenarios are described and discussed in terms of how the Code might be used to offer guidance to psychologists about how they might best respond. 相似文献
819.
关于知情同意的伦理与法律意义之辨析 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
刘俊荣 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2005,(9)
尊重和自主是知情同意的伦理底线,行善是知情同意的伦理基础。知情同意并不是评判医疗行为是否符合伦理的金标准。伦理上的知情同意与法律上的知情同意,既有区别又相互联系,德法并济是有效履行知情同意的前提。 相似文献
820.
Baum WM 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1993,59(2):245-264
Two differences between ratio and interval performance are well known: (a) Higher rates occur on ratio schedules, and (b) ratio schedules are unable to maintain responding at low rates of reinforcement (ratio “strain”). A third phenomenon, a downturn in response rate at the highest rates of reinforcement, is well documented for ratio schedules and is predicted for interval schedules. Pigeons were exposed to multiple variable-ratio variable-interval schedules in which the intervals generated in the variable-ratio component were programmed in the variable-interval component, thereby “yoking” or approximately matching reinforcement in the two components. The full range of ratio performances was studied, from strained to continuous reinforcement. In addition to the expected phenomena, a new phenomenon was observed: an upturn in variable-interval response rate in the midrange of rates of reinforcement that brought response rates on the two schedules to equality before the downturn at the highest rates of reinforcement. When the average response rate was corrected by eliminating pausing after reinforcement, the downturn in response rate vanished, leaving a strictly monotonic performance curve. This apparent functional independence of the postreinforcement pause and the qualitative shift in response implied by the upturn in variable-interval response rate suggest that theoretical accounts will require thinking of behavior as partitioned among at least three categories, and probably four: postreinforcement activity, other unprogrammed activity, ratio-typical operant behavior, and interval-typical operant behavior. 相似文献