全文获取类型
收费全文 | 9111篇 |
免费 | 1238篇 |
国内免费 | 520篇 |
专业分类
10869篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 41篇 |
2023年 | 218篇 |
2022年 | 217篇 |
2021年 | 217篇 |
2020年 | 500篇 |
2019年 | 466篇 |
2018年 | 438篇 |
2017年 | 467篇 |
2016年 | 419篇 |
2015年 | 297篇 |
2014年 | 385篇 |
2013年 | 1122篇 |
2012年 | 236篇 |
2011年 | 362篇 |
2010年 | 302篇 |
2009年 | 376篇 |
2008年 | 441篇 |
2007年 | 491篇 |
2006年 | 528篇 |
2005年 | 441篇 |
2004年 | 375篇 |
2003年 | 289篇 |
2002年 | 238篇 |
2001年 | 156篇 |
2000年 | 126篇 |
1999年 | 133篇 |
1998年 | 113篇 |
1997年 | 87篇 |
1996年 | 67篇 |
1995年 | 71篇 |
1994年 | 59篇 |
1993年 | 45篇 |
1992年 | 43篇 |
1991年 | 25篇 |
1990年 | 28篇 |
1989年 | 30篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 145篇 |
1984年 | 127篇 |
1983年 | 106篇 |
1982年 | 88篇 |
1981年 | 67篇 |
1980年 | 77篇 |
1979年 | 69篇 |
1978年 | 102篇 |
1977年 | 67篇 |
1976年 | 50篇 |
1975年 | 32篇 |
1974年 | 24篇 |
1973年 | 28篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
Shrinkage estimation of linear combinations of true scores 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Nicholas T. Longford 《Psychometrika》1997,62(2):237-244
This paper is concerned with combining observed scores from sections of tests. It is demonstrated that in the presence of population information a linear combination of true scores can be estimated more efficiently than by the same linear combination of the observed scores. Three criteria for optimality are discussed, but they yield the same solution which can be described and motivated as a multivariate shrinkage estimator.Input from Eric Bradlow, Charles Lewis, and Linda Zeger is acknowledged. Research for this paper was funded by the Program Research Council (ETS). Suggestions of the Editor and of anonymous referees were instrumental in several improvements of the paper. 相似文献
52.
William Grassie 《Zygon》1997,32(3):415-421
This essay is a discussion of effective teaching in the science-and-religion classroom. I begin by introducing Alfred North Whitehead's three stages of learning—romance, discipline, and generalization—and consider their implications for powerful pedagogy in science and religion. Following Whitehead's three principles, I develop a number of additional heuristics that deal with active, visual, narrative, cooperative, and dialogical learning styles. Finally, I present twelve guidelines for how to use e-mail and class-based listserves to achieve some of these outcomes. 相似文献
53.
Anna Case-Winters 《Zygon》1997,32(3):351-375
Both science and theology have lately faced a crisis of authority. Their shared realization of the extent to which knowledge is underdetermined by the data and socially constructed provides a kind of common ground for reconsideration of their respective methods of inquiry as well as of the status of the claims they have warrant to make. Both fields are now consciously and critically employing a models approach. This article proposes criteria for assessing models and applies the criteria to one model from each field. The model of understanding evolution as a struggle for existence is considered from the field of science, and the traditional model for understanding the God-world relation as that of a king's relation to his kingdom is considered from the field of theology. Each of these models is evaluated with respect to its credibility, religious viability, and moral adequacy. In each case an alternative analogy is proposed and argued for. 相似文献
54.
William Grassie 《Zygon》1997,32(1):83-94
This essay is an introduction to postmodernism and deconstruction as they relate to the special challenges of scholarship and teaching in the science and religion multidiscipline. 相似文献
55.
Matteo Selvini 《Contemporary Family Therapy》1997,19(3):315-335
The author examines the position of a child from whom important matters in his or her own life or key events in the family have been kept secret, outlines criteria by which the decision whether or not to unveil the secret should be made, and urges colleagues to gather more data and examine further family secrets. 相似文献
56.
Studies in East European Thought - 相似文献
57.
Kimihiko Yamagishi 《The Japanese psychological research》1997,39(2):124-129
Frequency estimation of social facts in two methods of judgment elicitation was investigated. In the “narrow-range” condition, subjects answered questions in the format: “Out of 100 incidents, how many belong to category X?” In the “wide-range” condition, the frequency for the same event was assessed with respect to “Out of 10,000”. Judged frequencies in the wide-range condition were divided by 100, and were compared with the corresponding judgments in the narrow-range condition. Such comparisons were made for low-frequency and high-frequency events. Previous research has shown that, for low-frequency events, judged frequencies are proportionally greater in the narrow-range than in the wide-range condition. These results reflect cognitive processes of implicit anchoring, whereby judged frequencies lie close to small numbers within the response ranges provided. I call this process “downward anchoring,” and predicted that this tendency would be replicated in the present study. Moreover, I predicted that assessments about high-frequency events would evoke similar cognitive processes operating in the opposite direction. By such “upward anchoring,” judged frequencies would lie close to relatively larger numbers within the given response ranges. Consequently, I predicted that judged frequencies for high-frequency events would be proportionally greater in the wide-range condition than in the narrow-range condition. These predictions were confirmed. 相似文献
58.
John Beebe 《The Journal of analytical psychology》1997,42(1):3-20
As a keynote to a conference bringing together psychoanalysts and analytical psychologists, this paper addresses different mythic attitudes toward the unconscious, Starting with the caricatures of Oedipus and Narcissus that the author feels Jung and Freud originally projected onto each Other in the course of their quarrel. He moves on to the fairytale-like stories of Perseus and Beauty and the Beast to discover mote complex images of the stance taken in relation to the unconscious by present-day analysts: working within both die Jungian and the Freudian traditions. 相似文献
59.
Three students with moderate disabilities were taught to read and match-to-sample sight words comprising stimulus sets based upon the four food groups. We taught students conditional discriminations within four four-member sets using a single-sample/four-comparison procedure. Students were taught A-B, B-C, and C-D conditional discriminations for each of the four potential stimulus classes. Subsequent probes tested for relations based upon symmetry and one-node and two-node transitivity. The performances for all students indicated that symmetric relations emerged before one-node transitive relations, and that one-node transitive relations emerged before two-node transitive relations. These results are consistent with a pattern of responding, referred to as a "nodality effect," in which relations with fewer nodes are demonstrated prior to the demonstration of relations with a greater number of nodes. These results extend this area of research to sight-word reading for students with moderate disabilities. 相似文献
60.
BALANCING SOCIAL ACCEPTABILITY WITH TREATMENT EFFECTIVENESS OF AN INTRUSIVE PROCEDURE: A CASE REPORT
Nancy C. Grace Sung Woo Kahng Wayne W. Fisher 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1994,27(1):171-172
The debate over social acceptability of intrusive procedures has led some school districts to adopt policies allowing punishment for only the most extreme forms of destructive behavior. We investigated the effectiveness of selectively implementing punishment for only the most severe topographies of aggression and property destruction, while less extreme behaviors were ignored. Results indicated that severe behaviors were reduced to near-zero levels only when both severe and less severe behaviors were similarly punished. 相似文献