全文获取类型
收费全文 | 138篇 |
免费 | 38篇 |
国内免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
188篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 9篇 |
2017年 | 10篇 |
2016年 | 6篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 6篇 |
2013年 | 12篇 |
2012年 | 6篇 |
2011年 | 4篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 10篇 |
2007年 | 4篇 |
2006年 | 13篇 |
2005年 | 2篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 8篇 |
2002年 | 5篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有188条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
论深化医疗卫生事业改革的伦理基础——兼谈《关于深化医药卫生体制改革的意见(征求意见稿)》 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
长期以来,我国医疗卫生事业改革发展受多方面因素的冲击与影响,陷入伦理精神缺失、公益性质淡化、价值目标扭曲、整体效率低下的窘境。深化医药卫生体制改革应当全面加强医疗卫生事业的伦理基础建设,努力实现行业伦理精神回归。新医改只有将解决当前群众反映强烈的“看病难、看病贵”的最大现实难题与合理规划长远发展战略相协调,着重从微观层面上提出切中要害的可操作性实施方案、监管制度、政策法规措施,才能切实引导医疗卫生事业摆脱困境,实现又好又快发展。 相似文献
172.
Antonella Lopez Alessandro O. Caffò Albert Postma Andrea Bosco 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2020,61(5):607-615
Spatial relations between landmarks can be represented by means of categories and coordinates. In the present research, this paradigm was applied to sketch maps based on information acquired in goal-directed behaviour of exploration of a university campus area. The first aim was to investigate whether categorical and coordinate information can be considered conceptually independent in sketch maps. The second aim was to assess which kind of distance measure served better to represent coordinate information in the present case study, and finally to assess the factorial structure of coordinate and categorical data. Analytic methodology as well as statistical analysis were found to confirm that separating coordinate and categorical components was formally as well as empirically appropriate. A series of confirmatory factor analyses showed the best fit for the model with two correlated components, as well as an acceptable reliability of measures emerged. The two components were moderately correlated. Moreover, the adoption of Manhattan distance seemed to be the most effective method to represent coordinate spatial relations in spatial sketch maps of areas acquired through navigation. 相似文献
173.
Kyungbong Kim 《亚洲哲学》2017,27(3):189-209
This study proposed to compare and analyze the five stages of cultivating the Yogâcāra path (唯識五位) and the spiritual journey in the Ten Ox-Herding Pictures (十牛圖). To achieve this, the study investigated the core concepts and practice methods of the two approaches and analyzed their relations from the literatures reviewed. The results showed that the end goal of the two approaches is the same, the attainment of Buddhahood, with the two having common characteristics including the practice of being aware of the impermanent and no-self (無常·無我), and the fulfilling benefit of sentient beings. The results suggest that our Buddhist practice system needs to sincerely consider the realistic ways by which one can help people in agony in contemporary society, not by emphasizing on the enlightenment through a specific practice way for all people, but by tailored practice methods based on each one’s faculties in understanding Buddhism. 相似文献
174.
The present study focuses on the semantic organisation of material in working memory. We developed a new measure in which students memorised unrelated words from lists. In our study, we manipulated the association between words in the lists. The material was organised so as to elicit a semantic organisation (categorical and thematic). The task was then administered to a group of 6–10-year-old children. The semantic organisation of the material prompted a better recall, which depended on the type of semantic organisation. In the same vein, the number of intrusion errors was influenced by the semantic links between words and was higher when words in the list were associated categorically. These results seemed to depend partly on the participants’ age, being evident only in the younger children. 相似文献
175.
Mariko Takagishi Michel van de Velden Hiroshi Yadohisa 《The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology》2019,72(3):401-425
Preference data, such as Likert scale data, are often obtained in questionnaire-based surveys. Clustering respondents based on survey items is useful for discovering latent structures. However, cluster analysis of preference data may be affected by response styles, that is, a respondent's systematic response tendencies irrespective of the item content. For example, some respondents may tend to select ratings at the ends of the scale, which is called an ‘extreme response style’. A cluster of respondents with an extreme response style can be mistakenly identified as a content-based cluster. To address this problem, we propose a novel method of clustering respondents based on their indicated preferences for a set of items while correcting for response-style bias. We first introduce a new framework to detect, and correct for, response styles by generalizing the definition of response styles used in constrained dual scaling. We then simultaneously correct for response styles and perform a cluster analysis based on the corrected preference data. A simulation study shows that the proposed method yields better clustering accuracy than the existing methods do. We apply the method to empirical data from four different countries concerning social values. 相似文献
176.
Many models of the Simon effect assume that categorical spatial representations underlie the phenomenon. The present study tested this assumption explicitly in two experiments, both of which involved eight possible spatial positions of imperative stimuli arranged horizontally on the screen. In Experiment 1, the eight stimulus locations were marked with eight square boxes that appeared at the same time during a trial. Results showed gradually increasing Simon effects from the central locations to the outer locations. In Experiment 2, the eight stimulus locations consisted of a combination of three frames of spatial reference (hemispace, hemifield, and position relative to the fixation), with each frame appearing in different timings. In contrast to Experiment 1, results showed an oscillating pattern of the Simon effect across the horizontal positions. These findings are discussed in terms of grouping factors involved in the Simon task. The locations seem to be coded as a single continuous dimension when all are visible at once as in Experiment 1, but they are represented as a combination of the lateral categories (“left” vs. “right”) with multiple frames of reference when the reference frames are presented successively as in Experiment 2. 相似文献
177.
178.
179.
180.