全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3624篇 |
免费 | 379篇 |
国内免费 | 220篇 |
专业分类
4223篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 17篇 |
2023年 | 128篇 |
2022年 | 49篇 |
2021年 | 116篇 |
2020年 | 187篇 |
2019年 | 224篇 |
2018年 | 161篇 |
2017年 | 213篇 |
2016年 | 200篇 |
2015年 | 183篇 |
2014年 | 182篇 |
2013年 | 547篇 |
2012年 | 106篇 |
2011年 | 175篇 |
2010年 | 130篇 |
2009年 | 169篇 |
2008年 | 191篇 |
2007年 | 162篇 |
2006年 | 184篇 |
2005年 | 141篇 |
2004年 | 127篇 |
2003年 | 120篇 |
2002年 | 121篇 |
2001年 | 70篇 |
2000年 | 60篇 |
1999年 | 34篇 |
1998年 | 43篇 |
1997年 | 20篇 |
1996年 | 36篇 |
1995年 | 30篇 |
1994年 | 26篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 15篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有4223条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
运动员的生活满意感:个人自尊与集体自尊的贡献 总被引:40,自引:0,他引:40
评估了个人自尊与集体自尊在预测运动员的一般生活满意感和训练比赛满意感时的相对重要性。被试为139名中国运动员,男68名,女71名,平均年龄19.64岁。分层回归的结果发现,在控制了人口统计学变量、个人自尊和社会支持3类变量以后,集体自尊单独解释了训练比赛满意感总方差的13%。优势分析的结果表明,预测训练比赛满意感时,集体自尊最为重要,贡献了已解释方差的67%。这些结果提示,需要用不同的自尊来预测不同的满意感。 相似文献
82.
Diego Gomez‐Baya Marta Reis Margarida Gaspar de Matos 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2019,60(6):559-568
The framework of Positive Youth Development (PYD) arose from a strength‐based conception of the transition to adulthood. Although previous literature has provided evidence for some PYD correlates, little is known about gender differences in PYD and its outcomes in thriving and social engagement, which could improve interventions” effectiveness. Thus, this study aimed to examine gender differences in PYD dimensions (i.e., competence, confidence, connection, caring, and character), individual thriving (i.e., psychological adjustment, academic adjustment, and healthy lifestyles) and social engagement, and to examine the associations between them. Data from the ‘PYD in a Cross‐National Perspective Project” in Spain were collected by administering a paper‐based self‐report to students from high school and university. Results showed greater scores in connection, caring, and character in women, as well as greater social engagement and academic adjustment. Men presented higher competence and confidence, more frequent physical activity and better psychological adjustment. In addition, more PYD was related to greater social engagement, better psychological and academic adjustment, and healthier lifestyles. Some practical implications for program design may be derived, which underscore the need to promote the five PYD dimensions equally in female and male youths in order to improve their individual thriving and social engagement. 相似文献
83.
Erik Goodwyn 《The Journal of analytical psychology》2019,64(5):720-737
During the course of the 2018 IAAP conference, a criticism of Jung’s idea of the archetype as inherited predisposition was raised that involved examining a number of dreams and visions and assessing them through developments in genetics and neuroscience. From this comparison it was argued that archetypes cannot be inherited and could more reasonably be argued to derive from early experiences. In this essay, the author responds by showing how this conclusion is flawed due to being based on reductive errors. An alternative, non‐reductive but inherited and biological position on the archetype is defended. 相似文献
84.
Joan Biruls Laura Bosch Ricarda Brieke Ferran Pons David J. Lewkowicz 《Developmental science》2019,22(3)
Previous findings indicate that bilingual Catalan/Spanish‐learning infants attend more to the highly salient audiovisual redundancy cues normally available in a talker's mouth than do monolingual infants. Presumably, greater attention to such cues renders the challenge of learning two languages easier. Spanish and Catalan are, however, rhythmically and phonologically close languages. This raises the possibility that bilinguals only rely on redundant audiovisual cues when their languages are close. To test this possibility, we exposed 15‐month‐old and 4‐ to 6‐year‐old close‐language bilinguals (Spanish/Catalan) and distant‐language bilinguals (Spanish/”other”) to videos of a talker uttering Spanish or Catalan (native) and English (non‐native) monologues and recorded eye‐gaze to the talker's eyes and mouth. At both ages, the close‐language bilinguals attended more to the talker's mouth than the distant‐language bilinguals. This indicates that language proximity modulates selective attention to a talker's mouth during early childhood and suggests that reliance on the greater salience of audiovisual speech cues depends on the difficulty of the speech‐processing task. 相似文献
85.
Joan Paul Pozuelos Lina M. Combita Alicia Abundis Pedro M. Paz‐Alonso ngela Conejero Sonia Guerra M. Rosario Rueda 《Developmental science》2019,22(2)
Interventions including social scaffolding and metacognitive strategies have been used in educational settings to promote cognition. In addition, increasing evidence shows that computerized process‐based training enhances cognitive skills. However, no prior studies have examined the effect of combining these two training strategies. The goal of this study was to test the combined effect of metacognitive scaffolding and computer‐based training of executive attention in a sample of typically developing preschoolers at the cognitive and brain levels. Compared to children in the regular training protocol and an untrained active control group, children in the metacognitive group showed larger gains on intelligence and significant increases on an electrophysiological index associated with conflict processing. Moreover, changes in the conflict‐related brain activity predicted gains in intelligence in the metacognitive scaffolding group. These results suggest that metacognitive scaffolding boosts the influence of process‐based training on cognitive efficiency and brain plasticity related to executive attention. 相似文献
86.
Christine E. Potter Eva Fourakis Elizabeth Morin‐Lessard Krista Byers‐Heinlein Casey Lew‐Williams 《Developmental science》2019,22(4)
In bilingual language environments, infants and toddlers listen to two separate languages during the same key years that monolingual children listen to just one and bilinguals rarely learn each of their two languages at the same rate. Learning to understand language requires them to cope with challenges not found in monolingual input, notably the use of two languages within the same utterance (e.g., Do you like the perro? or ¿Te gusta el doggy?). For bilinguals of all ages, switching between two languages can reduce the efficiency in real‐time language processing. But language switching is a dynamic phenomenon in bilingual environments, presenting the young learner with many junctures where comprehension can be derailed or even supported. In this study, we tested 20 Spanish–English bilingual toddlers (18‐ to 30‐months) who varied substantially in language dominance. Toddlers’ eye movements were monitored as they looked at familiar objects and listened to single‐language and mixed‐language sentences in both of their languages. We found asymmetrical switch costs when toddlers were tested in their dominant versus non‐dominant language, and critically, they benefited from hearing nouns produced in their dominant language, independent of switching. While bilingualism does present unique challenges, our results suggest a united picture of early monolingual and bilingual learning. Just like monolinguals, experience shapes bilingual toddlers’ word knowledge, and with more robust representations, toddlers are better able to recognize words in diverse sentences. 相似文献
87.
Jelena Obradovi Jenna E. Finch Ximena A. Portilla Muneera A. Rasheed Nicole Tirado‐Strayer Aisha K. Yousafzai 《Developmental science》2019,22(5)
This study extends the methodological and theoretical understanding of executive functions (EFs) in preschoolers from low‐ and middle‐income countries (LMIC). First, the authors describe a rigorous process of adapting and evaluating six EF tasks to produce a culturally and developmentally appropriate measure of emerging EFs in a large sample of at‐risk children in rural Pakistan. Next, the authors identify critical developmental and family factors that relate to preschoolers’ EFs over the first 4 years of life. Direct assessment of children's general cognitive skills at age two showed developmental continuity with EFs at age four, and these early cognitive skills mediated the effect of an antecedent parenting intervention on EFs as well as associations of targeted individual and family factors with EFs. Furthermore, directly assessed maternal cognitive capacities and observed maternal scaffolding uniquely predicted EFs in preschoolers. This study is also the first to demonstrate a significant overlap between direct assessments of IQ and EFs in young children from LMIC. Children's general intelligence mediated the associations of EFs with antecedent physical growth and cognitive skills as well as concurrent family factors (maternal verbal intelligence, maternal scaffolding, and home stimulation). After controlling for shared variance between preschoolers’ general intelligence and EFs, three factors emerged as unique predictors of EFs: exposure to an early parenting intervention, physical growth status at age two, and number of older siblings. The findings have important implications for the design of interventions that aim to improve EFs in young children in LMIC. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://vimeo.com/316329544/5abde94cd7 相似文献
88.
Kenneth S. Shultz Deborah A. Olson Eric M. Vogelsang 《Adultspan: Theory Research & Practice》2019,18(2):99-111
An increasing number of older workers are attempting career changes; however, little is known about the antecedents or outcomes of these changes. Results indicate that perceived objective and perceived subjective career‐change success are predicted by different combinations of variables. Implications for both future research and practice are discussed. 相似文献
89.
90.
Massage techniques and postural manipulation used by West African women immigrants in Paris were studied, with the aim of demonstrating that culturally regulated caretaking practices have an influence on infant motor development. Twenty-five infants were observed between the ages of 3 and 12 months and 9 and 15 months. Frequency of massage and its accompanying set of physical exercises were found to be related to age at onset of independent walking. The findings show that infants receiving daily massage walk significantly earlier than infants who do not receive massage, thus confirming other reports that vestibular and postural stimulation have a positive effect on motor development. These results are discussed in relation to the observed frequencey of contact and of physical games with the caregiver for a smaller sample of three infants, and to parental expectations about the age of acquisition of certain cognitive and motor abilities. African immigrant mothers expect their infants to sit and to walk earlier than French mothers. The conclusion emphasizes the contribution of social anthropology, which shows that in all cultures, physical development and body changes elicit symbolic practices and a search for explanation. 相似文献