全文获取类型
收费全文 | 411篇 |
免费 | 82篇 |
国内免费 | 26篇 |
专业分类
519篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 3篇 |
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 15篇 |
2022年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 25篇 |
2019年 | 33篇 |
2018年 | 21篇 |
2017年 | 24篇 |
2016年 | 18篇 |
2015年 | 9篇 |
2014年 | 25篇 |
2013年 | 56篇 |
2012年 | 22篇 |
2011年 | 18篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 24篇 |
2008年 | 16篇 |
2007年 | 15篇 |
2006年 | 13篇 |
2005年 | 14篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 10篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 11篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有519条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
221.
R. Duncan Luce 《Psychometrika》1977,42(4):461-489
Four issues are discussed concerning Thurstone's discriminal processes: the distributions governing the representation, the
nature of the response decision rules, the relation of the mean representation to physical characteristics of the stimulus,
and factors affecting the variance of the representation. A neural schema underlying the representation is proposed which
involves samples in time of pulse trains on individual neural fibers, estimators of parameters of the several pulse trains,
samples of neural fibers, and an aggregation of the estimates over the sample. The resulting aggregated estimate is the Thurstonian
representation. Two estimators of pulse rate, which is monotonic with signal intensity, are timing and counting ratios and
two methods of aggregation are averaging and maximizing. These lead to very different predictions in a speed-accuracy experiment;
data indicate that both estimators are available and the aggregation is by averaging. Magnitude estimation data are then used
both to illustrate an unusual response rule and to study the psychophysical law. In addition, the pattern of variability and
correlation of magnitude estimates on successive trials is interpreted in terms of the sample size over which the aggregation
takes place. Neural sample size is equated with selective attention, and is an important factor affecting the variability
of the representation. It accounts for the magical number seven phenomenon in absolute identification and predicts the impact
of nonuniform distributions of intensities on the absolute identification of two frequencies.
1977 Psychometric Society Presidential Address.
This work was supported in part by a grant of the National Science Foundation to Harvard University. I wish to express my
appreciation to S. Burbeck, D. M. Green, M. Shaw, and B. Wandell for their useful comments on an earlier draft of this paper. 相似文献
222.
The relationship between auditory temporal processing,phonemic awareness,and reading disability 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
Recent research suggests an auditory temporal deficit as a possible contributing factor to poor phonemic awareness skills. This study investigated the relationship between auditory temporal processing of nonspeech sounds and phonological awareness ability in children with a reading disability, aged 8-12 years, using Tallal's tone-order judgement task. Normal performance on the tone-order task was established for 36 normal readers. Forty-two children with developmental reading disability were then subdivided by their performance on the tone-order task. Average and poor tone-order subgroups were then compared on their ability to process speech sounds and visual symbols, and on phonological awareness and reading. The presence of a tone-order deficit did not relate to performance on the order processing of speech sounds, to poorer phonological awareness or to more severe reading difficulties. In particular, there was no evidence of a group by interstimulus interval interaction, as previously described in the literature, and thus little support for a general auditory temporal processing difficulty as an underlying problem in poor readers. In this study, deficient order judgement on a nonverbal auditory temporal order task (tone task) did not underlie phonological awareness or reading difficulties. 相似文献
223.
The face inversion effect, or impaired recognition of upside down compared to upright faces, is used as a marker for the configural
processing of faces in primates. The inversion effect in humans and chimpanzees is strongest for categories of stimuli for
which subjects have considerable expertise, primarily conspecifics’ faces. Moreover, discrimination performance decreases
linearly as faces are incrementally rotated from upright to inverted. This suggests that rotated faces must be transformed,
or normalized back into their most typical viewpoint before configural processing can ensue, and the greater the required
normalization, the greater the likelihood of errors resulting. Previous studies in our lab have demonstrated a general face
inversion effect in rhesus monkeys that was not influenced by expertise. Therefore, the present study examined the influence
of rotation angle on the visual perception of face and nonface stimuli that varied in their level of expertise to further
delineate the processes underlying the inversion effect in rhesus monkeys. Five subjects discriminated images in five orientation
angles. Results showed significant linear impairments for all stimulus categories, including houses. However, compared to
the upright images, only rhesus faces resulted in worse performance at rotation angles greater than 45°, suggesting stronger
configural processing for stimuli for which subjects had the greatest expertise. 相似文献
224.
Horowitz A 《Animal cognition》2009,12(1):107-118
The social cognitive capacities of dogs, including their communication skills and use of visual attention cues, have recently
been investigated in numerous experimental studies. This paper reports on research of domestic dog behavior in a natural setting,
which shows sensitivity to the visual attention of their partners when engaged in dyadic rough-and-tumble play. The sequential
behaviors and head-direction of both dogs were noted throughout the bouts. The behaviors were differentially used according
to the partner’s posture. Play signals were sent nearly exclusively to forward-facing conspecifics; attention-getting behaviors
were used most often when a playmate was facing away, and before signaling an interest to play. In addition, the mode of attention-getter
matched the degree of inattentiveness of the playmate: stronger attention-getters were used when a playmate was looking away
or distracted, less forceful ones when the partner was facing forward or laterally. In other words, these dogs showed attention
to, and acted to manipulate, a feature of other dogs that mediates their ability to respond: which feature in human interaction
is called “attention”.
相似文献
Alexandra HorowitzEmail: |
225.
226.
学习不良儿童的元认知偏差:内部线索的影响 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
通过改变实验材料内部线索,旨在考察内部线索在学习不良儿童元认知偏差形成过程中的影响。实验1发现,学习不良儿童低关联字对的元认知偏差显著高于一般儿童,关联强度对学习判断的影响显著小于对测试成绩的影响;实验2发现,学习不良儿童反向方向字对的元认知偏差显著高于一般儿童,关联方向对学习判断的影响显著小于对测试成绩的影响。综合以上研究发现,相对一般儿童,学习不良儿童存在更大的元认知偏差,内部线索对学习判断和测试成绩的影响不一致模式适用于两组儿童。 相似文献
227.
使用联接和离散的线索呈现方式比较了注意的易化效应和返回抑制的效果。实验一同时线索化3个外周位置,实验二同时线索化5个外周位置。结果表明:(1)在同时线索化3个位置的情境下,联接和离散这两种线索化方式均能产生返回抑制;(2)在同时线索化5个位置的情境下,联接条件仍能产生抑制效应,而离散条件没有产生。结果说明联接的方式让多个位置的线索被理解为一个线索,提示注意有“沿着物体弥散”的性质。 相似文献
228.
The effects of chronic, daily administration of cocaine on auditory and visual reaction times and thresholds were studied in baboons. Single intramuscular injections of cocaine hydrochloride (0.1 to 5.6 mg/kg) were given once daily for periods of 10 to 25 days, and were followed immediately by psychophysical tests designed to assess cocaine's effects on simple reaction times as on auditory and visual threshold functions. Consistent reductions in reaction times were frequently observed over the cocaine dose range of 0.32 to 1.0 mg/kg; at higher doses, either decreases or increases in reaction times were observed, depending upon the animal. Lowered reaction times generally occurred immediately following the 1st day's cocaine injection, and continued through all subsequent days during the dose administration period, suggesting little development of tolerance or sensitivity to these reaction-time effects. Reaction-time decreases showed a U-shaped dose-effect function. The greatest decreases in reaction times occurred from 0.32 to 1.0 mg/kg, and produced an average reaction-time decrease of 10 to 12%. Concurrently measured auditory and visual thresholds showed no systematic changes as a function of cocaine dose. Pausing was observed during performance of the psychophysical tasks, with the length of total session pause times being directly related to cocaine dose. 相似文献
229.
Judith A. Hall Sarah D. GunnerySusan A. Andrzejewski 《Journal of research in personality》2011,45(1):77-83
The goal of this research was to investigate the impact of nonverbal expressive cues on the attribution of the Big Five personality traits. Expressive cues of fear, disgust, happiness, and sadness were elicited in a sample of 22 encoders while watching films, narrating, and posing. Encoders’ personalities were rated by themselves and unacquainted raters who watched the encoders, and blind judges rated the traits of a typical student. Expressive cues influenced the raters’ attribution of personality, but this influence was weakest when the encoders expressed happiness (vs. negative emotions) and when they were narrating an emotional experience (when the cues were least potent). Negative and strong expressive cues interfered with the application of a normative, and more accurate, judgment strategy. 相似文献
230.
Because the choice to trust is inherently risky, people naturally assess others’ trustworthiness before they engage in trusting actions. The research reported here suggests that the trust development process may start before the conscious assessment of trustworthiness, via the activation of a relational schema. We present three experiments that examined the automatic, non-conscious activation of interpersonal trusting behavior via a variety of subliminal cues: positive or negative, relational or non-relational, and trust-related or not. In all three studies, subliminal relational cues influenced subsequent trusting behavior, apparently without conscious awareness. Results from the third study also indicated that subliminal relational cues that were specifically trust-related influenced trustors’ expectations of the likelihood of reciprocity. Overall, the data provide initial evidence that the development of interpersonal trust can start before and beneath conscious awareness. 相似文献