全文获取类型
收费全文 | 371篇 |
免费 | 31篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
403篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 18篇 |
2018年 | 16篇 |
2017年 | 18篇 |
2016年 | 14篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 46篇 |
2012年 | 12篇 |
2011年 | 11篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 20篇 |
2007年 | 25篇 |
2006年 | 25篇 |
2005年 | 19篇 |
2004年 | 16篇 |
2003年 | 13篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有403条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
两种人本心理学的辩论 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
该文沿着JohnRowan(1989)提出是否有两种人本心理学这一问题 ,追溯了人本心理学内部分歧的由来 ,即罗洛·梅与罗杰斯在 2 0世纪 80年代初有关人性问题的争论 ,以及 1986年罗洛·梅对超个人心理学的批评。文章着重介绍了维尔伯 (KenWilber)对这一领域的分歧所做的全面分析 ,指出了两种人本心理学在一些基本问题上的不同立场。这两种心理学就是以罗洛·梅为代表的存在主义取向的人本心理学和以马斯洛、罗杰斯、维尔伯为代表的从自我实现走向自我超越的人本心理学 相似文献
22.
Martin Robra 《The Ecumenical review》2019,71(5):755-770
Plurality of life forms and value systems is a given (almost) everywhere on earth. Imposing on them a value system or individual values and virtues from an exterior position is rather counterproductive. Value systems are normative for individual communities but not the same. They are to relate to each other and to interact, negotiating with each other common goals and the validity and limits of values involved in their relationships and shared action. Anything else leads to tensions and violence. Internormative ethics reflect on such interaction of value systems. 相似文献
23.
Arthur Stamps III Ph.D. 《Science and engineering ethics》1997,3(1):85-98
This paper presents a framework that editors, peer reviewers, and authors can use to identify and resolve efficiently disputes
that arise during peer review in scientific journals. The framework is called a scientific dialectical brief. In this framework,
differences among authors and reviewers are formatted into specific assertions and the support each party provides for its
position. A literature review suggests that scientists use five main types of support; empirical data, reasoning, speculation,
feelings, and status. It is suggested that the scientific dialectical brief format can streamline the review process by facilitating
rapid differentiation between stronger and weaker support, so that valuable time can be focused on the better-substantiated
claims. The paper concludes with some suggestions for implementation.
The author researches, lectures and publishes in the area of physical and social aspects of environmental quality. He is also
a practicing architect.
This paper is based on a presentation at a workshop, “Advances in Peer Review Research”, American Association for the Advancement
of Science Meeting, Baltimore, MD, February 9, 1996. 相似文献
24.
Walter Mischel studied self-control in preschool children in the following manner: if the child waited for an interval to end, he or she received the more preferred of two reinforcers; if the child responded to terminate the interval by ringing a bell, the less preferred reinforcer was given. We used an analogous procedure to study self-control in pigeons: if the bird waited for a trial to end, it received the more preferred reinforcer; if the bird terminated the trial by pecking a key, the less preferred reinforcer was given. We explored the effects on self-control of a number of variables analogous to those studied by Mischel and co-workers, e.g., presence versus absence of reinforcers, of alternative responses, and of stimuli during the wait interval; prior experience of the subjects; and test paradigm. The results obtained with pigeons paralleled the results obtained by Mischel with human children. 相似文献
25.
Alternative response training, differential reinforcement of other behavior, and extinction in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus)
下载免费PDF全文

Mulick JA Leitenberg H Rawson RA 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1976,25(3):311-320
In Experiment I, (a) extinction, (b) extinction plus reinforcement of a discrete alternative response, and (c) differential reinforcement of other behavior were each correlated with a different stimulus in a three-component multiple schedule. The alternative-response procedure more rapidly and completely suppressed behavior than did differential reinforcement of other behavior. Differential reinforcement of other behavior was slightly more effective than extinction alone. In Experiment II, reinforcement of specific alternative behavior during extinction and differential reinforcement of other behavior were used in two components, while one component continued to provide reinforcement for the original response. Once again, the alternative-response procedure was most effective in reducing responding as long as it remained in effect. However, the responding partially recovered when reinforcement for competing behavior was discontinued. In general, responding was less readily reduced by differential reinforcement of other behavior than by the specific alternative-response procedure. 相似文献
26.
27.
Bertram D. Cohen 《Group》2002,26(3):189-204
The group self, an individual's self-perception as a member of a group, is distinguished from the group's self, a personification of the group as a person with a self. The perceived group's self of an opposing other group is often a repository for attributes group members are not ready to recognize in their own group's self. Theoretically, the need for an adversarial other to hold one's own group's unwanted attributes—and thus to complete one's own group's self—contributes, however unconsciously, to the apparent intractability of intergroup disputes. In dealing with these issues, I compare intergroup conflict resolution (ICR) groups with therapy groups, on the one hand, and advocacy groups on the other. All of this sets the stage for a presentation of two ICR projects and their analysis in group-developmental terms. 相似文献
28.
Patricia Flynn Weitzman Eben A. Weitzman Sue E. Levkoff 《Journal of Clinical Geropsychology》2002,8(2):139-147
Older women may respond to conflicts by suppressing anger. Suppressed anger is associated with depression and hypertension. Research on the everyday conflicts of community-dwelling older women has begun to emerge. Such information is not available for women in nursing homes, and is necessary for developing interventions that can help protect women in nursing homes against anger-related illness. Nursing home administrators were surveyed to evaluate women's health promotion programming in general, and the incidence of and institutional response to conflicts of women residents. About 74% of residents were women; yet, women's health promotion programs were rare (offered in 1 out of 25 facilities, or 4%). Ninety percent of administrators identified depression as the most significant women's health problem, but tended to see it as warranting medical rather than programmatic attention. Eighty percent identified strained interpersonal relationships as a significant health problem for women, requiring programmatic attention. In particular, administrators identified the need for programming to help women residents resolve conflicts with roommates and family members. Formal mechanisms for managing conflicts of residents did not exist in sample nursing homes. 相似文献
29.
尤吾兵 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2007,28(9):38-39
医德是医学道德的简称,具言之,它是指医务人员在医疗卫生服务的职业活动中应该具备的仁爱、救死扶伤等的品德,是道德德目在医务人员身上的内化,也是对医务人员必须具备的最起码的要求.我们评价一个医生,首先是直面他的医德,医德是医生形象的体现,没有医德的医生即使技艺再高超,也算不上一个完整意义上的医生,"无德不成医".事实上一个医德低下的医生,他的技艺也不会高超的. 相似文献
30.
Irmen L 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》2007,36(6):431-456
Two eye-tracking studies assessed effects of grammatical and conceptual gender cues in generic role name processing in German. Participants read passages about a social or occupational group introduced by way of a generic role name (e.g., Soldaten/soldiers, Künstler/artists). Later in the passage the gender of this group was specified by the anaphoric expression diese Männer/these men or diese Frauen/these women. Testing masculine generic role names of male, female or neutral conceptual gender (Exp. 1) showed that a gender mismatch between the role name’s conceptual gender and the anaphor significantly slowed reading immediately before and after the anaphoric noun. A mismatch between the antecedent’s grammatical gender and the anaphor slowed down the reading of the anaphoric noun itself. Testing grammatically gender-unmarked role names (Exp. 2) revealed a general male bias in participants’ understanding, irrespective of grammatical or conceptual gender. The experiments extend previous findings on gender effects to non-referential role names and generic contexts. Theoretical aspects of gender and plural reference as well as gender information in mental models are discussed. 相似文献