全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2063篇 |
免费 | 136篇 |
国内免费 | 22篇 |
专业分类
2221篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 1篇 |
2024年 | 27篇 |
2023年 | 46篇 |
2022年 | 28篇 |
2021年 | 90篇 |
2020年 | 114篇 |
2019年 | 188篇 |
2018年 | 185篇 |
2017年 | 190篇 |
2016年 | 118篇 |
2015年 | 122篇 |
2014年 | 68篇 |
2013年 | 341篇 |
2012年 | 46篇 |
2011年 | 49篇 |
2010年 | 21篇 |
2009年 | 54篇 |
2008年 | 55篇 |
2007年 | 56篇 |
2006年 | 71篇 |
2005年 | 58篇 |
2004年 | 42篇 |
2003年 | 40篇 |
2002年 | 22篇 |
2001年 | 22篇 |
2000年 | 19篇 |
1999年 | 24篇 |
1998年 | 18篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有2221条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
The roles of belief in a just world (BJW) and discrimination against ones' group in perceptions of personal discrimination were examined. Female participants (n = 63) were personally discriminated against in a laboratory setting. We manipulated whether the experimenter appeared to discriminate against other participants, which presumably made the presence of personal discrimination less ambiguous, or did not appear to discriminate against others, which presumably made personal discrimination more ambiguous. In the no group discrimination condition, but not in the group discrimination condition, participants' perceptions of being personally discriminated against depended on individual differences in BJW: Women with a strong BJW perceived less personal discrimination than those with a weak BJW. Also, strong BJW women in the group discrimination condition perceived less personal discrimination than strong BJW women in the no group discrimination condition. 相似文献
132.
15名被试在摄人酒精BAL≤0.5‰前后的四个时段内接受一套简单的运算操作任务,近2/3被试的认知一操作能力显示受酒精影响发生变异:酒后30分钟操作时间延滞,至60分钟更具显著性;同时,操作错误亦大幅上升,但变化趋势不同于操作时间。酒精干扰呈现认知“近视”与“远视”的迹象。 相似文献
133.
职前教师与在职教师在算术应用题上真实性思考的研究 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
采用由7道真实性算术应用题构成的测试材料研究了117名职前教师和72名在职教师,首先要求被试解答这些题目,然后评定学生的不同答案。结果表明:1)职前教师与在职教师表现出真实性思考的多重性。2)我国职前教师被试的真实性思考的人数比例为87.5%,高于国外研究所报告的比例。3)在职教师的真实性思考、真实性解答的人数比例都高于职前教师,但没有达到显著性差异;不确定的真实性解答的人数比例极显著地高于职前教师;常规性解答的人数比例显著地低于职前教师。在评定学生解答时、在职教师更愿意接受“不确定”这样一个解题的逻辑性结果,职前教师对常规性解答的态度更为宽容。 相似文献
134.
We designed this study to examine children's self-reported and observed emotional and behavioral responding to marital conflict as a potential mechanism linking marital physical aggression (as reported by the parents) and children's behavioral adjustment (as reported by their preschool teachers). In a sample of 48 preschoolers, parental marital physical aggression was positively associated with children's observed dysregulated responding to interparental conflict and negatively associated with children's self-reported behavioral disruption. Marital aggression and children's self-reported responding to marital conflict predicted teacher-reported behavior problems, with both variables adding unique variance. Our findings suggested a potential pathway linking exposure to marital conflict, children's regulatory strategies, and children's behavioral adjustment outside the home. 相似文献
135.
记叙文阅读中时间心理表征的建构:动态观还是静态观 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
探讨读者在文本阅读中如何通过一系列展开事件的描述建构心理表征。实验一探讨在中间事件持续时间长/短条件下对首尾呼应句阅读时间和对探测词反应时间的影响;结果表明,读者对第一事件信息的获得受中间事件的持续时间长短的影响。实验二探讨当信息储存在长时工作记忆中时能否得出与实验一相一致的结果。结果发现,读者对第一事件信息的获得不受中间事件的持续时间长短的影响。实验三考察当用明确的时间副词来表明持续时间的长短时,连续性的事件是否还存在着时间距离效应。实验结果与实验二的相一致。整个研究结果表明,时间心理表征的建构是动态观还是静态观,主要取决于是否存在时间距离效应。 相似文献
136.
使用单任务研究程序,采用引入提示线索的方法,以产生时距作为反应指标对存在间断的时距估计任务中的间断期望效应和提示线索效应(注意效应)进行系统考察,并对间断时距的效应、产生时距与等待时距的关系问题作出进一步探讨。结果表明,间断位置(等待时距)因素是被试时间判断的主要线索,被试的产生时距随着等待时距的增加而延长。间断实验中表现出极其显著的提示线索效应,此效应既增加了时距估计的变异,又延长了被试的时距估计。无间断实验条件下,被试表现出显著的间断期望效应,被试对间断的期望有损于时间估计。 相似文献
137.
Tim Oakley 《Australasian journal of philosophy》2017,95(1):70-80
In some cases, you may release someone from some obligation they have to you. For instance, you may release them from a promise they made to you, or an obligation to repay money they have borrowed from you. But most take it as clear that, if you have an obligation to someone else, you cannot in any way release yourself from that obligation. I shall argue the contrary. The issue is important because one standard problem for the idea of having duties to oneself relies on the impossibility of self-release. The argument (the ‘Release Argument’) is that a duty to oneself would be a duty from which one could release oneself, but that this is an absurdity, and so there can be no duties to oneself. This argument is to be rejected because a duty from which one can release oneself is perfectly possible, and such release occurs quite properly from time to time. 相似文献
138.
139.
The aim of this study was to investigate stability and change over 26 years in self-reported adjustment of Swedish teenage girls. Data were collected with the same questionnaire from two school-cohorts in a middle-sized Swedish community: 522 girls attending Grade 8 (approximately at age 15) in 1970, and 529 girls attending Grade 8 in 1996. The first cohort was part of the longitudinal research programme Individual Development and Adaptation (IDA). In most domains, adjustment problems were approximately as common in 1996 as in 1970, with two exceptions: more girls reported problems with self-esteem and antisocial problems in 1996. In the antisocial domain, a polarization process was indicated, with an increase also in the number of girls without adjustment problems. In the relational domains, especially peer relations, there was an increase in positive adjustment. The results are discussed in relation to earlier findings and to social changes during the period. 相似文献
140.
高玉玲 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2005,26(3):71-72
针对2004年<医学与哲学>第7期孙福川教授<对"人造美人"医学的十大人文拷问>一文,提出自己的观点,认为"人造美人"是临床医疗行为,其出现并没有偏离医学的目的,"人造美人"的法律主体地位应是患者,因人造美人而出现的纠纷可以适用于处理医疗纠纷的相关法律、法规,同时造美广告也是医疗广告. 相似文献