全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2628篇 |
免费 | 226篇 |
国内免费 | 32篇 |
专业分类
2886篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 46篇 |
2023年 | 77篇 |
2022年 | 42篇 |
2021年 | 59篇 |
2020年 | 119篇 |
2019年 | 127篇 |
2018年 | 133篇 |
2017年 | 107篇 |
2016年 | 121篇 |
2015年 | 74篇 |
2014年 | 105篇 |
2013年 | 300篇 |
2012年 | 58篇 |
2011年 | 75篇 |
2010年 | 85篇 |
2009年 | 89篇 |
2008年 | 130篇 |
2007年 | 138篇 |
2006年 | 131篇 |
2005年 | 113篇 |
2004年 | 81篇 |
2003年 | 50篇 |
2002年 | 60篇 |
2001年 | 38篇 |
2000年 | 56篇 |
1999年 | 50篇 |
1998年 | 33篇 |
1997年 | 37篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 29篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 14篇 |
1990年 | 20篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 19篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 17篇 |
1979年 | 20篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 16篇 |
1976年 | 13篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有2886条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
251.
Mark Graves 《Zygon》2007,42(1):241-248
Cognitive science and religion provides perspectives on human cognition and spirituality. Emergent systems theory captures the subatomic, physical, biological, psychological, cultural, and transcendent relationships that constitute the human person. C. S. Peirce's metaphysical categories and existential graphs enrich traditional cognitive science modeling tools to capture emergent phenomena. From this richer perspective, one can reinterpret the traditional doctrine of soul as form of the body in terms of information as the constellation of constitutive relationships that enables real possibility. 相似文献
252.
Christopher C. Knight 《Theology & Science》2018,16(2):151-165
The understanding of the mind usually adopted within the current science-theology dialogue is questionable. It fails to take into account something necessary to provide a non-reductionist understanding of religious faith: what in ancient and medieval theology was termed the nous. While this concept may require re-interpretation for our present age in terms of a different philosophical framework, any recognition of its reality will have a major impact on two aspects of current discussion: our response to philosophical idealism and our understanding of the eschatological state. 相似文献
253.
Classroom Climate and Political Learning: Findings from a Swedish Panel Study and Comparative Data
下载免费PDF全文

Mikael Persson 《Political psychology》2015,36(5):587-601
Numerous studies have shown that an open classroom climate for discussion increases students' civic knowledge. However, most previous studies draw on cross‐sectional data and have not been able to show that the effect is causal. This article presents results from a Swedish panel survey following students during the first year in the gymnasium (upper secondary level). Using this study, we are better equipped to evaluate the link between an open classroom climate and political knowledge. Results suggest that the effect is causal. A 10% increase in open classroom climate is associated with about 5 percentage points higher knowledge. The beneficial effect of an open classroom climate is an important insight that should be seriously considered not only by researchers but also by educational policy makers, school managements, and teachers. 相似文献
254.
Garrett Kenney 《Zygon》2015,50(1):227-244
This article examines Huston Smith's critique of and remedy for modernity from the perspective of a college professor who adopted “Why Religion Matters” (2001) as required reading for undergraduates. Smith's heartfelt plea to consider, if not embrace, the common wisdom of traditional religious worldviews deserves a hearing. But Smith's approach is also in need of qualification, supplementation, and critique. This article, ironically, finds the needed qualification, supplementation, and critique in Huston Smith's much earlier publication, The Purposes of Higher Education (1955). This article provides the dialogue. 相似文献
255.
THE CRAZINESS FOR EXTRA‐SENSORY PERCEPTION: QIGONG FEVER AND THE SCIENCE–PSEUDOSCIENCE DEBATE IN CHINA
下载免费PDF全文

From 1979 to 1999, a heated dispute over the science or pseudoscience of extraordinary power or extrasensory perception (ESP) took place in China. During these two decades, many so‐called “grandmasters” of ESP and Qigong emerged, and millions of people across the country studied with them; this was known as “Qigong Fever” or “ESP Fever.” The supporters of ESP argued that ESP existed, people could cultivate ESP through specific Qigong training, and ESP was a science; whereas the opponents of ESP denied all of these. Both sides of the dispute had many supporters. With the onset of Qigong Fever in China, some Qigong and ESP masters developed their Qigong organizations into Chinese‐style religions. Qigong Fever ended when the religions were banned by the Chinese government. The rise of Qigong Fever demonstrated that basic questions about the boundaries between science and pseudoscience were not easy to answer. Different theoretical and practical consequences resulted from different answers to these questions. 相似文献
256.
James Bohman 《International Journal of Philosophical Studies》2013,21(3):353-377
Abstract My goal here is to come to terms with the Enlightenment as the horizon of critical social science. First, I consider in more detail the understanding of the Enlightenment in Critical Theory, particularly in its conception of the sociality of reason. Second, I develop an account of freedom in terms of human powers, along the lines of recent capability conceptions that link freedom to the development of human powers, including the power to interpret and create norms. Finally, I show the ways in which the social sciences can be moral sciences in the Enlightenment sense. This account provides us with a coherent Enlightenment standard by which to judge institutions as promoting development, understood in terms of the capabilities necessary for freedom. The relevant social science in this area might include the robust generalization that there has never been a famine in a democratic society. 相似文献
257.
Marc Lachieze‐Rey 《World Futures: Journal of General Evolution》2013,69(1-3):27-29
Education can only be effective if interdisciplinary and decompartmentalized. The young must have a scientific culture if they are to adapt to the contemporary world and to its evolution. Teaching science must be a way of training the mind to reasoning, to method, to discernment. Science nevertheless remains but a means, an aid for decision; scientific truth must be made relative by resting upon the history of science. 相似文献
258.
Robin H. Willemsen Isabelle C. de Vink Evelyn H. Kroesbergen Ard W. Lazonder 《创造性行为杂志》2024,58(1):137-150
This intervention study examined the effectiveness of instructional support tailored toward two techniques (i.e., random associations and constraint identification) to strengthen children's creative problem-solving skills within upper-elementary science education. Five inquiry-based science lessons with ample opportunity for creative problem-solving (i.e., divergent and convergent thinking) were provided. Children were assigned to a condition with instructional support (n = 107) or without (n = 134). Domain-general and specific measures of divergent and convergent thinking were included, and reading comprehension as well as mathematical ability were taken into account. Repeated measures multivariate analyses of covariance revealed how all children improved in terms of domain-general convergent thinking, with a larger increase for children who performed better in mathematics. This shows a promising premise for future research focusing on the domain generality of convergent thinking and for the potential of transfer across domains. No additional improvement based on instructional support was found and children did not improve in terms of divergent thinking. The constraint identification and random associations technique might not be suitable for elementary school children, yet future research is necessary to validate such claims. Meanwhile, teachers could possibly support convergent thinking by simply providing exercises for divergent and convergent thinking. 相似文献
259.
Tamara Kalandadze;Sara A. Hart; 《Infant and child development》2024,33(1):e2334
The increasing adoption of open science practices in the last decade has been changing the scientific landscape across fields. However, developmental science has been argued to be relatively slow in adopting open science practices. One of the barriers to applying open science practices might be a lack of knowing ‘how to start’ among developmental researchers. To address this issue, we followed the format of Crüwell et al. (2019) and created summaries and an annotated list of informative and actionable resources discussing ten topics in developmental science: Open science practices; Issues with reproducibility and replication; Open data, materials and code; Open access; Preregistration; Registered reports; Replication; Incentives; Collaborative developmental science. This article offers researchers in developmental science a starting point for understanding how open science intersects with developmental science. After getting familiarized with this article, developmental scientists should understand the core tenets of open and reproducible developmental science, and feel motivated to start applying open science practices in their workflow. 相似文献
260.
This study examines how the types of language expression and message valence used by teachers to convey classroom-compliance instructions affect elementary-school children's psychological reactance and their intention to follow instructions. Altogether, 178 elementary-school children (54% girls) participated in a mixed-design experiment. Participants read four self-developed cartoons, where a hypothetical teacher conveyed classroom-compliance instructions through four expressions, and responded to questions measuring psychological reactance and the intention to follow instructions based on the cartoons. The results showed that psychological reactance was relatively significant in the high-controlling language condition. In the low-controlling language condition, the approach evoked less psychological reactance and enhanced the intention to follow instructions, whereas avoidance did so in the high-controlling language condition. Our findings offer suggestions on how teachers can improve classroom management by altering their messages in accordance with the motivation characteristics of their students. 相似文献