全文获取类型
收费全文 | 686篇 |
免费 | 71篇 |
国内免费 | 19篇 |
专业分类
776篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 4篇 |
2024年 | 12篇 |
2023年 | 17篇 |
2022年 | 13篇 |
2021年 | 18篇 |
2020年 | 26篇 |
2019年 | 20篇 |
2018年 | 31篇 |
2017年 | 37篇 |
2016年 | 53篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 30篇 |
2013年 | 141篇 |
2012年 | 24篇 |
2011年 | 51篇 |
2010年 | 24篇 |
2009年 | 30篇 |
2008年 | 34篇 |
2007年 | 25篇 |
2006年 | 16篇 |
2005年 | 22篇 |
2004年 | 17篇 |
2003年 | 18篇 |
2002年 | 21篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 1篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有776条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
671.
Biological rhythms play a prominent role in the modulation of human physiology and behavior. [Smith, K., Valentino, D., & Arruda, J. (2003). Rhythmic oscillations in the performance of a sustained attention task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 561-570] suggested that sustained human performance may systematically fluctuate in a cyclic manner with periods of 1.5 min and 5.2 min. The current series of investigations sought to manipulate those periodicities by altering task difficulty, administering caffeine, and testing on a more ecologically valid task. Strong evidence of a 1.5 min periodicity was found across studies. Most participants did not demonstrate the 5.2 min periodicity. Moreover, the 1.5 min periodicity was resistant to task manipulations and appeared in similar levels across conditions in all three experiments. These rhythms may be indicative of an endogenous system that modulates sustained attention in humans. Evidence supporting this idea and implications of the research are discussed. 相似文献
672.
Desmond J. Leach Steven G. Rogelberg Peter B. Warr Jennifer L. Burnfield 《Journal of business and psychology》2009,24(1):65-76
Purpose The aim of this investigation was to test hypotheses about meeting design characteristics (punctuality, chairperson, etc.)
in relation to attendees’ perceptions of meeting effectiveness.
Design/Methodology/Approach Two studies were conducted: Study 1 investigated meetings attended in a typical week (N = 958), whereas Study 2 examined the last meeting attended on a particular day (N = 292).
Findings A number of design characteristics (in particular agenda use and quality of facilities) were found to be important in predicting
perceived effectiveness. Attendee involvement served as a key mediator variable in the observed relationships. Neither meeting
type nor size was found to affect the relationships of the design characteristics and involvement with effectiveness. Meeting
size, however, was negatively related to attendee involvement.
Implications The findings help us to better understand relationships between design characteristics and attendees’ perceptions of meeting
effectiveness. Meeting organizers can use the findings to guide administration of meetings, with potential to enhance the
quality of meetings.
Originality/Value Meetings are a common organizational activity but are rarely the focus of empirical research. The use of two complementary
studies, to our knowledge, provides a unique account of the contribution of design characteristics to perceptions of meeting
effectiveness.
Received and reviewed by former editor, George Neuman. 相似文献
673.
Carmen Picazo Nuria Gamero Jose M. Peiró 《European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology》2013,22(2):297-307
Based on a two-dimensional perspective of group cohesion, this study examines the emergence of task cohesion and interpersonal cohesion in project teams and their roles in changes in members’ individual satisfaction with the team. Specifically, we tested a direct-effect and mediation model of the cross-level relationship between team task and interpersonal cohesion and individual satisfaction with the team over time. With a sample of 74 newly created project teams, the hypotheses were tested using a two-wave panel design. Results indicate that task cohesion emerges more strongly than interpersonal cohesion during the first stages of work in project teams. Moreover, the cross-lagged relationship between team interpersonal cohesion and individual satisfaction with the team was mediated by team task cohesion. 相似文献
674.
LENE FALGAARD EPLOV JANNE PETERSEN TORBEN JøRGENSEN CHRISTOFFER JOHANSEN MORTEN BIRKET‐SMITH ANN CHRISTINE LYNGBERG ERIK LYKKE MORTENSEN 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2010,51(6):548-554
Eplov, L.F., Petersen, J., Jørgensen, T., Johansen, C., Birket‐Smith, M., Lyngberg, A. C. & Mortensen, E. L. (2010). The Mental Vulnerability Questionnaire: A psychometric evaluation. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 51, 548–554. The Mental Vulnerability Questionnaire was originally a 22 item scale, later reduced to a 12 item scale. In population studies the 12 item scale has been a significant predictor of health and illness. The scale has not been psychometrically evaluated for more than 30 years, and the aim of the present study was both to evaluate the psychometric properties of the 22 and 12 item scales and of three new scales. The main study sample was a community sample comprising more than 6,000 men and women. In this sample the coefficients of homogeneity were all over 0.30 for the three new scales, but below 0.30 for the 12 and the 22 item scales. All five Mental Vulnerability scales had positively skewed score distributions which were associated significantly with both SCL‐90‐R symptom scores and NEO‐PI‐R personality scales (primarily Neuroticism and Extraversion). Coefficient alpha was highest for the 22 and 12 item scales, and the two scales also showed the highest long‐term stability. The three new scales reflect relatively independent dimensions of Psychosomatic Symptoms, Mental Symptoms, and Interpersonal Problems, but because of reliability problems it remains an open question whether they will prove useful as predictors of health and morbidity. 相似文献
675.
《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2013,66(8):1599-1614
Recent studies have revealed that verbal representations play an important role in various task-switching situations. This study examined whether verbal representations contribute to the actual switching process using random task cueing with two cues per task. This procedure allowed us to produce a trial in which the cue switched, but the task repeated, thereby separating the cue-switching process from the actual task-switching process. Participants performed colour or shape judgements that were initiated by an arbitrary symbol cue (Experiments 1 and 2) or a kanji cue (Experiment 3) under control, articulatory-suppression, and foot-tapping conditions. In Experiments 1 and 2 with the arbitrary cues, articulatory suppression impaired performance in only the cue-switch condition. In Experiment 3, in which a kanji cue indicated the upcoming task name, articulatory suppression did not have any effects. These results suggest that the involvement of verbal representations in random task cueing is based on the cue-switching process rather than on the task-switching process. 相似文献
676.
《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2013,66(11):2255-2273
Investigations into the persistence of top-down control settings do not accurately reflect the nature of dynamic tasks. They typically involve extended practice with an initial task, and this initial task usually shares similar stimuli with a second task. Recent work shows that visual attention and search can be affected by limited exposure to a preceding, unrelated task, and the current study explored the temporal characteristics of this “carry-over” effect. Thirty-four participants completed one, four, or eight simple letter searches and then searched a natural scene. The spatial layout of letters influenced spread of search in the pictures, and this was further impacted by the time spent in the initial task, yet the carry-over effect diminished quickly. The results have implications for theories of top-down control and models that attempt to predict search in natural scenes. They are also relevant to real-world tasks in which performance is closely related to visual attention and search. 相似文献
677.
《European Journal of Developmental Psychology》2013,10(4):476-494
Research on bilingualism has shown a bilingual advantage in phonological and syntactic awareness, and more recently on some morphological awareness tasks in children who had acquired two languages simultaneously. However, there is still limited knowledge about this advantage in students who acquire a second language in school after having developed a first language. The present longitudinal study addressed this issue by comparing, over the first two years of primary school, the development of metalinguistic abilities in 33 French learners enrolled in an immersion French–German programme to that of 43 French monolingual peers. Results support the bilingual advantage and add to the growing body of research reaching the same conclusion. In this study, this advantage appears as early as in first grade and the gap favouring second language learners widens with time. The results are discussed in light of differences between the characteristics of the two languages. 相似文献
678.
《Journal of Cognitive Psychology》2013,25(4):468-475
We investigated implicit task sequence learning with auditory stimuli. In previous studies only visual stimuli have been used and thus learning may have been due to visuoperceptual learning. Further, we explored the generality of the correlated streams account which holds that correlated streams of information are necessary for implicit sequence learning to occur. We used three classification tasks with auditory stimuli. The presence or absence of a task sequence was orthogonally manipulated with that of a response sequence. Sequence-specific learning was found, but only in the condition with both a task and a response sequence. No learning was found in the conditions with a single task sequence and with a single response sequence. These results show that task–response sequence learning occurs with auditory stimuli and that visuoperceptual learning is not necessary. Moreover, they underscore the importance of correlated streams of information for implicit sequence learning. 相似文献
679.
Leila Selimbegovic Isabelle Régner Pascal Huguet 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2011,47(1):72-78
Two studies tested the impact of autobiographical recall of general versus specific academic success or failure on actual task performance. As expected, it was found that general memories of failure and specific memories of success resulted in worse performance than general memories of success and specific memories of failure. In Study 1, this performance pattern was obtained on a standard math test. In Study 2, it was replicated on a test of intellectual ability, and a mediation by fear of failure was documented. The present findings offer direct evidence that autobiographical memories of success and failure impact actual performance and also reveal the role of memory specificity in this influence. 相似文献
680.
为考察不同行为激活系统/行为抑制系统下的个体在Iowa赌博任务反应模式下的决策行为,本研究采用人格问卷与行为实验相结合的方式,探讨了人格、冲动和行为决策间的关系。结果发现:(1)行为激活系统三个维度与行为决策存在显著负相关,行为激活系统低分组的IGT得分较高,行为激活系统高分组的IGT得分较低,而冲动特征与IGT得分相关不显著;(2)行为激活系统高、低分者在IGT任务上差异显著,且行为激活系统高分组者在收益-风险并存情境中表现出"即时收益优先"的决策模式。结果提示,在收益-风险并存情境中,高行为激活的人格特征者倾向风险决策模式。 相似文献