全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3322篇 |
免费 | 681篇 |
国内免费 | 273篇 |
专业分类
4276篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 17篇 |
2024年 | 45篇 |
2023年 | 71篇 |
2022年 | 105篇 |
2021年 | 118篇 |
2020年 | 175篇 |
2019年 | 167篇 |
2018年 | 162篇 |
2017年 | 209篇 |
2016年 | 235篇 |
2015年 | 114篇 |
2014年 | 202篇 |
2013年 | 702篇 |
2012年 | 179篇 |
2011年 | 233篇 |
2010年 | 121篇 |
2009年 | 205篇 |
2008年 | 201篇 |
2007年 | 184篇 |
2006年 | 155篇 |
2005年 | 135篇 |
2004年 | 112篇 |
2003年 | 98篇 |
2002年 | 68篇 |
2001年 | 42篇 |
2000年 | 31篇 |
1999年 | 32篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 22篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有4276条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
We report experiments based on a novel test in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus), designed to examine the encoding of two different geometric features of an enclosed environment: relative lengths of the walls and amplitude of the corners. Chicks were trained to search for a food reward located in one corner of a parallelogram-shaped enclosure. Between trials, chicks were passively disoriented and the enclosure was rotated, making reorientation possible only on the basis of the internal spatial structure of the enclosure. In order to reorient, chicks could rely on two sources of information: the relative lengths of the walls of the enclosure (associated to their left-right sense order) and the angles subtended by walls at corners. Chicks learned the task choosing equally often the reinforced corner and its rotational equivalent. Results of tests carried out in novel enclosures, the shapes of which were chosen ad hoc (1) to induce reorientation based only on the ratio of walls lengths plus sense (rectangular enclosure), or (2) to induce reorientation based only on corner angles (rhombus-shaped enclosure), suggested that chicks encoded both features of the environment. In a third test, in which chicks faced a conflict between these geometric features (mirror parallelogram-shaped enclosure), reorientation seemed to depend on the salience of corner angles. These results shed light on the elements of the environmental geometry which control spatial reorientation, and broaden the knowledge on the geometric representation of space in animals. 相似文献
152.
新的人身损害赔偿司法解释将于2004年5月1日正式施行.预计将引起医疗纠纷诉讼大量增加,且多以医疗侵权为诉因,医疗机构的赔偿费用也会大幅上升.医疗行业应从减少医疗过失,改善医患关系,完善医疗事故鉴定等方面加以应对. 相似文献
153.
不同年龄广告名人效应的心理加工机制研究 总被引:3,自引:1,他引:3
本研究从自动化加工和控制性加工这个视角研究了名人效应的心理加工机制。实验运用加工分离程序,探讨了不同年龄阶段的受众对名人广告的信息加工模式。结果发现:(1)名人效应是一个普遍存在的现象,与用一般消费者做代言人相比,青少年、成年人(大学生)以及老年人对名人广告均明显存在更多的自动化加工,且加工水平没有年龄差异。但是,老年人的控制性加工明显低于其他两组;(2)在非注意条件下,代言人的专业化程度明显地影响了大学生组的控制性加工,但与各组被试的自动化加工都关系不大。这些研究发现为广告策略的制定带来了一些新的启示。 相似文献
154.
Ziegler JC Perry C Ma-Wyatt A Ladner D Schulte-Körne G 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2003,86(3):169-193
Most of the research on developmental dyslexia comes from English-speaking countries. However, there is accumulating evidence that learning to read English is harder than learning to read other European orthographies (Seymour, Aro, & Erskine, 2003). These findings therefore suggest the need to determine whether the main English findings concerning dyslexia can be generalized to other European orthographies, all of which have less irregular spelling-to-sound correspondences than English. To do this, we conducted a study with German- and English-speaking children (n=149) in which we investigated a number of theoretically important marker effects of the reading process. The results clearly show that the similarities between dyslexic readers using different orthographies are far bigger than their differences. That is, dyslexics in both countries exhibit a reading speed deficit, a nonword reading deficit that is greater than their word reading deficit, and an extremely slow and serial phonological decoding mechanism. These problems were of similar size across orthographies and persisted even with respect to younger readers that were at the same reading level. Both groups showed that they could process larger orthographic units. However, the use of this information to supplement grapheme-phoneme decoding was not fully efficient for the English dyslexics. 相似文献
155.
影响产品体积知觉的形状效应研究 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
产品包装是现代营销的重要手段之一,也是消费与广告心理学领域的研究热点之一。产品包装的形状、尺寸和体积大小都会影响到消费者的判断、购买决策和消费行为。本文就影响产品体积知觉的高径比效应(elongation),从其概念、形状消费错觉与形重错觉的关系、实际消费中的体积知觉效应,以及注意对形状效应的影响等方面进行了阐述,并作了简要的评论。 相似文献
156.
Non-human animals and human children can make use of the geometric shape of an environment for spatial reorientation and in
some circumstances reliance on purely geometric information (metric properties of surfaces and sense) can overcome the use
of local featural cues. Little is known as to whether the use of geometric information is in some way reliant on past experience
or, as would likely be argued by advocates of the notion of a geometric module, it is innate. We tested the navigational abilities
of newborn domestic chicks reared in either rectangular or circular cages. Chicks were trained in a rectangular-shaped enclosure
with panels placed at the corners to provide salient featural cues. Rectangular-reared and circular-reared chicks proved equally
able to learn the task. When tested after removal of the featural cues, both rectangular- and circular-reared chicks showed
evidence that they had spontaneously encoded geometric information. Moreover, when trained in a rectangular-shaped enclosure
without any featural cues, chicks reared in rectangular-, circular-, or c-shaped cages proved to be equally able to learn
and perform the task using geometric information. These results suggest that effective use of geometric information for spatial
reorientation does not require experience in environments with right angles and metrically distinct surfaces, thus supporting
the hypothesis of a predisposed geometric module in the animal brain. 相似文献
157.
Three experiments demonstrate gender congruency effects (i.e., naming times of a picture are faster when the name of the target
picture and a distractor noun are gender congruent) in Czech. In the first experiment, subjects named the pictures by producing
gender-marked demonstrative pronouns and a noun. In the second and third experiments, subjects produced a gender-marked numeral
(marked with a suffix) plus a noun. Two types of such suffixes exist in Czech. Some numerals vary in nominative singular with
gender, others do not. The results show significant gender congruency effects in all experiments. They suggest that gender
congruency effects can be obtained not only with free, but also with bound morphemes. In the second and third experiment the
effect only emerged when the suffix was gender-marked (as opposed to gender-invariant), supporting the view that the gender
congruency effect is due to competition at the level of phonological forms rather than at the grammatical level. 相似文献
158.
159.
采用无线索回忆再认范式,对基于熟悉性的汉字语义特征再认进行了探索,考察了重复学习和重复测验对汉字语义特征的无线索回忆再认效应(RWCR效应)的影响。实验1采用即时测验,实验2采用延时测验,结果发现:(1)无论即时还是延时测验,汉字语义特征的再认均存在RWCR效应。(2)在即时测验时,重复学习对熟悉性有显著影响,重复测验对熟悉性没有影响;重复学习和重复测验均提高回想成绩,但二者无差异。(3)在延时测验时,重复学习组和重复测验组的熟悉性评分均下降,但前者下降快于后者;重复测验组回想的遗忘率较低,重复学习组回想的遗忘率较高。上述结果说明,汉字的语义特征存在稳定的RWCR效应,且重复学习主要影响熟悉性,重复测验主要影响回想。进一步证明了再认记忆的双加工理论。 相似文献
160.
Languages differ in how they encode spatial frames of reference. It is unknown how children acquire the particular frame-of-reference terms in their language (e.g., left/right, north/south). The present paper uses a word-learning paradigm to investigate 4-year-old English-speaking children’s acquisition of such terms. In Part I, with five experiments, we contrasted children’s acquisition of novel word pairs meaning left-right and north-south to examine their initial hypotheses and the relative ease of learning the meanings of these terms. Children interpreted ambiguous spatial terms as having environment-based meanings akin to north and south, and they readily learned and generalized north-south meanings. These studies provide the first direct evidence that children invoke geocentric representations in spatial language acquisition. However, the studies leave unanswered how children ultimately acquire “left” and “right.” In Part II, with three more experiments, we investigated why children struggle to master body-based frame-of-reference words. Children successfully learned “left” and “right” when the novel words were systematically introduced on their own bodies and extended these words to novel (intrinsic and relative) uses; however, they had difficulty learning to talk about the left and right sides of a doll. This difficulty was paralleled in identifying the left and right sides of the doll in a non-linguistic memory task. In contrast, children had no difficulties learning to label the front and back sides of a doll. These studies begin to paint a detailed account of the acquisition of spatial terms in English, and provide insights into the origins of diverse spatial reference frames in the world’s languages. 相似文献