首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1236篇
  免费   157篇
  国内免费   172篇
  1565篇
  2024年   2篇
  2023年   20篇
  2022年   27篇
  2021年   51篇
  2020年   57篇
  2019年   67篇
  2018年   68篇
  2017年   69篇
  2016年   56篇
  2015年   58篇
  2014年   74篇
  2013年   140篇
  2012年   50篇
  2011年   51篇
  2010年   45篇
  2009年   76篇
  2008年   72篇
  2007年   76篇
  2006年   64篇
  2005年   55篇
  2004年   58篇
  2003年   33篇
  2002年   41篇
  2001年   21篇
  2000年   23篇
  1999年   17篇
  1998年   29篇
  1997年   16篇
  1996年   16篇
  1995年   19篇
  1994年   17篇
  1993年   10篇
  1992年   12篇
  1991年   5篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   6篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   8篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   7篇
  1982年   6篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   5篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   2篇
排序方式: 共有1565条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
971.
Patients with left unilateral spatial neglect (USN) typically place the subjective midpoint to the right of the objective centre. Based on the previous findings (e.g., Ishiai et al. 1989, Brain, 112, 1485), we hypothesized that the patients with left USN may see the representational image of a line that extends equally towards either side of the subjective midpoint depending not upon the information about the leftward extent. The present study tested whether patients with left USN would place the subjective midpoint at the centre of their mental representation of the line. The participants were 10 patients with left USN and 10 neurologically healthy controls. We devised a new ‘endpoint reproduction task’ using a computer display with a touch panel to seek the representational image when patients with left USN bisect lines and asked the participants to reproduce the location of the right or left endpoint after bisecting lines. The results showed that the representational image of the bisected line depends primarily on the location of the objective right endpoint, not on the location of the objective left endpoint in space. The analyses of the estimated right and left representational extents confirmed our hypotheses that patients with left USN would bisect a line seeing the representational line image that centred across their subjective midpoint. We believe that the findings of the present study with the use of the endpoint reproduction task will contribute to a better understanding of the visuospatial process underlying line bisection of patients with left USN.  相似文献   
972.
本研究采取前后测实验设计,以内隐联想测验和Buss的攻击性问卷为研究工具,探讨了媒体暴力对大学生攻击性的长时效应和短时效应。结果发现:(1)个体实验前接触媒体暴力的程度越强烈,内隐攻击性越强,外显攻击性也越强;(2)接触暴力视频游戏10分钟就会增强个体的内隐攻击性,接触非暴力视频游戏10分钟对个体的内隐攻击性没有影响;接触暴力和非暴力视频游戏10分钟对个体的外显攻击性没有影响;(3)外显攻击性基线水平和游戏类型的交互作用不显著;内隐攻击性基线水平和游戏类型交互作用显著。内隐攻击性基线水平较高的个体,游戏类型对其内隐攻击性的影响差异不显著;内隐攻击性基线水平较低的个体,暴力视频游戏增加了他们的内隐攻击性,非暴力视频游戏对他们的内隐攻击性没有影响。  相似文献   
973.
吴锐  丁树良  甘登文 《心理学报》2010,42(3):434-442
题组越来越多地出现在各类考试中, 采用标准的IRT模型对有题组的测验等值, 可能因忽略题组的局部相依性导致等值结果的失真。为解决此问题, 我们采用基于题组的2PTM模型及IRT特征曲线法等值, 以等值系数估计值的误差大小作为衡量标准, 以Wilcoxon符号秩检验为依据, 在几种不同情况下进行了大量的Monte Carlo模拟实验。实验结果表明, 考虑了局部相依性的题组模型2PTM绝大部分情况下都比2PLM等值的误差小且有显著性差异。另外, 用6种不同等值准则对2PTM等值并评价了不同条件下等值准则之间的优劣。  相似文献   
974.
注意缺损多动障碍量表的编制   总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7       下载免费PDF全文
 本研究在综合国内外有关儿童行为问题的测量问卷的基础上,编制了适合我国小学生特点的《儿童注意缺损多动障碍量表》。经过预测后的修订,组成正式量表。共计34个题目,施测于5所普通小学1-6年级的433名被试。因素分析结果验证了本量表五个因子的存在,即:注意力缺损、多动、冲动、唤醒不足、角色管理失控行为,从而验证了最初的结构预想。本研究着重考察了该量表的信度和效度:其中总量表与各分量表的同质性信度在.9202-.9448之间,总量表的再测信度为.8902。对量表结构效度和实证效度的检验也取得了令人满意的结果。  相似文献   
975.
In the present study, we examined intra‐individual relations among emotions when receiving test results and subsequent test review activities, and the direct and moderating effects of perceptions of test value. The participants, 90 first‐year students at a private girls' secondary school, self‐reported their present emotional state immediately after receiving their test results and their test review activities 1 week after the tests were returned. The same survey was conducted on four occasions. Results of multilevel analyses indicated that emotions experienced when receiving test results did not predict subsequent learning behavior. The results also showed that relations among emotions and review activities differed across students. Although we investigated whether such individual differences in relations might be attributed to perceptions of test value, no moderating effect could be observed. Furthermore, improvement‐oriented test mindsets were found to be positive predictors of review activities by investigating the direct effect of perceptions of test value. These findings suggest that modifying students' perceptions of test value may promote appropriate learning behavior.  相似文献   
976.
The present article is concerned with a common misunderstanding in the interpretation of statistical mediation analyses. These procedures can be sensibly used to examine the degree to which a third variable (Z) accounts for the influence of an independent (X) on a dependent variable (Y) conditional on the assumption that Z actually is a mediator. However, conversely, a significant mediation analysis result does not prove that Z is a mediator. This obvious but often neglected insight is substantiated in a simulation study. Using different causal models for generating Z (genuine mediator, spurious mediator, correlate of the dependent measure, manipulation check) it is shown that significant mediation tests do not allow researchers to identify unique mediators, or to distinguish between alternative causal models. This basic insight, although well understood by experts in statistics, is persistently ignored in the empirical literature and in the reviewing process of even the most selective journals.  相似文献   
977.
978.
以广东省佛山市"升中"考试为例.分析和探讨如何选用合适的等值设计与方法来解决普教"升中"考试不同地区分数转换的问题.采用非随机组锚测验等值设计对三种经典测验等值方法进行比较.结果发现:Tuck-er线性等值方法最优,kvine线性等值方法次之,等百分位等值方法(频数估计)不适合此类等值.等值方差分析表明题型与等值方法具有交互作用,这说明不同的题型宜选用不同的等值方法来进行等值.  相似文献   
979.
The relation between morality and the brain is a topic usefully examined through the evolutionary neurology of John Hughlings-Jackson, who considered higher mental function to be progressively inclusive integration of sensori-motor processes. His view, based on careful observations of patients with neurological disorders, implies that moral reasoning involves integration and coordination of behaviour through a process of representation and re-representation encompassing broader and broader types of information sensitive to environmental contingencies. The relevant information is processed in diverse brain areas: superior temporal sulcus (STS), inferior parietal lobule (IPL), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), dorsolateral prefrontal (DLPF) areas, as well as anterior temporal (AT) structures. Moral function can be regarded as maximally integrating emotion, social cognition, and other-regarding sensibilities using propositionally organised cognitive structures that map a shared world of human activity and relationships so that they take account of what in social and personal life counts as something.  相似文献   
980.
The fact that engaging in a cognitive task before a recognition task increases the probability of “old” responses is known as the revelation effect. We used several cognitive tasks to examine whether the feeling of resolution, a key construct of the occurrence mechanism of the revelation effect, is related to the occurrence of the revelation effect. The results show that the revelation effect was not caused by a visual search task, which elicited the feeling of resolution, but caused by an unsolvable anagram task and an articulatory suppression task, which did not elicit the feeling of resolution. These results suggest that the revelation effect is not related to the feeling of resolution. Moreover, the revelation effect was likely to occur in participants who performed poorly on the recognition task. The result suggests that the revelation effect is inclined to occur when people depend more on familiarity than on recollection process.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号