全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3466篇 |
免费 | 324篇 |
国内免费 | 129篇 |
专业分类
3919篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 6篇 |
2024年 | 80篇 |
2023年 | 76篇 |
2022年 | 87篇 |
2021年 | 136篇 |
2020年 | 183篇 |
2019年 | 238篇 |
2018年 | 160篇 |
2017年 | 199篇 |
2016年 | 181篇 |
2015年 | 121篇 |
2014年 | 137篇 |
2013年 | 529篇 |
2012年 | 94篇 |
2011年 | 175篇 |
2010年 | 90篇 |
2009年 | 153篇 |
2008年 | 175篇 |
2007年 | 130篇 |
2006年 | 139篇 |
2005年 | 141篇 |
2004年 | 110篇 |
2003年 | 104篇 |
2002年 | 93篇 |
2001年 | 52篇 |
2000年 | 52篇 |
1999年 | 47篇 |
1998年 | 32篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 23篇 |
1995年 | 23篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 16篇 |
1992年 | 11篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 9篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有3919条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
The second offspring of General Process Learning Theory: Overt behavior as the ambassador of the mind 总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0
下载免费PDF全文

Malone JC 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1982,38(2):205-209
Wasserman suggested in a recent book review that the study of intervening cognitive processes represents a current focus of interest in animal learning and that this has led to a revitalization of comparative psychology. An examination of the volume reviewed suggests that he may have overstated the case. Most of the authors to whom he refers expressed dissatisfaction with traditional stimulus-response associationism but few argued for the extreme (information processing) sort of cognitive approach described by Wasserman. 相似文献
12.
J A Dinsmoor K L Mueller L T Martin C A Bowe 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1982,38(3):249-263
Pigeons were exposed to stimuli correlated with the presence or absence of a variable-interval 60-second schedule of reinforcement only while they depressed a crossbar or "perch." In the first experiment, the stimuli were different tilts of a line displayed on the key. When the difference in brightness between the line and the background (salience) was maximal, seven of eight birds acquired the discrimination, but when the difference was reduced by 50%, only one succeeded. In the second experiment, wavelength of chamber illumination served as the relevant dimension. Neither experiment showed a large effect attributable to the magnitude of the difference (disparity) between the positive and the negative stimulus. Individual differences in time spent observing were positively correlated with level of discrimination in the presence of the stimuli. All birds produced the positive stimulus for a greater proportion of the available time than they did the negative stimulus. This may be the mechanism that provides selective reinforcement of observing. Finally, the formation of a discrimination was analyzed in terms of changes in the proportion of time spent in contact with the discriminative stimuli. 相似文献
13.
R O Straub M S Seidenberg T G Bever H S Terrace 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1979,32(2):137-148
Three pigeons learned to peck four colors in a particular sequence, regardless of how these colors were positioned on four response keys and without feedback following each response. This demonstrates that serial learning is possible in subprimate animals. 相似文献
14.
This study investigated whether an observed inverse relationship between self-stimulation and learning in autistic children is due to a physical inability of the subject to use the same body part for self-stimulation and task responding, or whether self-stimulation “distracts” the subject from task responding. Four actively self-stimulating autistic children were taught two discrimination tasks; one required a response that physically interferred with their self-stimulation; the other did not. Results were: (a) all subjects responded similarly across both tasks; (b) the three subjects with higher mental age scores learned both tasks without external suppression of self-stimulation; and (c) none of the subjects showed an inverse relationship between self-stimulation and learning. The study demonstrated that elimination of self-stimulation is not a necessary prerequisite for the acquisition of a new behavior in all autistic children. 相似文献
15.
Henry M. Halff 《Psychometrika》1976,41(3):301-320
Two forms of stationarity prior to criterion in absorbing Markov chains are examined. Both forms require that the probability of a particular response on a particular trial before absorption be independent of trial number. The stronger of these forms holds that this is true independent of starting state; the weaker, only for a specified set of starting probabilities. Simple, necessary and sufficient conditions for both forms are developed and applied to several examples.The author would like to thank Charles Lewis for his help in developing this article, and Peter Polson of the University of Colorado and an anonymous referee for several fruitful suggestions made in reviews of earlier versions. 相似文献
16.
学习不良青少年与一般青少年学业情绪特点的比较研究 总被引:14,自引:0,他引:14
本研究采用问卷法调查了1034名青少年(学习不良青少年506名),比较了学习不良青少年与一般青少年的学业情绪的特点。结果表明:(1)学习不良青少年的积极学业情绪显著低于一般青少年;学习不良青少年的消极学业情绪显著高于一般青少年。(2)学习不良青少年与一般青少年在学业情绪上的差异主要表现在初一、初二和高二、高三年级。(3)总体来说,男生的积极学业情绪多于女生,女生的消极学业情绪多于男生。 相似文献
17.
This paper overviews the interactivist model of representation and its applications in artificial intelligence (AI) and intelligent robotics. Selected examples from approaches in AI and robotics are contrasted with the generic interactivist architecture in order to illustrate specific features of it. Petitagé, an artificial agent that instantiates our interactivist-expectative theory of agency and learning (IETAL), is discussed in detail from the interactivist perspective. 相似文献
18.
Neural modeling and imaging of the cortical interactions underlying syllable production 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
This paper describes a neural model of speech acquisition and production that accounts for a wide range of acoustic, kinematic, and neuroimaging data concerning the control of speech movements. The model is a neural network whose components correspond to regions of the cerebral cortex and cerebellum, including premotor, motor, auditory, and somatosensory cortical areas. Computer simulations of the model verify its ability to account for compensation to lip and jaw perturbations during speech. Specific anatomical locations of the model's components are estimated, and these estimates are used to simulate fMRI experiments of simple syllable production. 相似文献
19.
Eighteen healthy young adults underwent event-related (ER) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) of the brain while performing a visual category learning task. The specific category learning task required subjects to extract the rules that guide classification of quasi-random patterns of dots into categories. Following each classification choice, visual feedback was presented. The average hemodynamic response was calculated across the eighteen subjects to identify the separate networks associated with both classification and feedback. Random-effects analyses identified the different networks implicated during the classification and feedback phases of each trial. The regions included prefrontal cortex, frontal eye fields, supplementary motor and eye fields, thalamus, caudate, superior and inferior parietal lobules, and areas within visual cortex. The differences between classification and feedback were identified as (i) overall higher volumes and signal intensities during classification as compared to feedback, (ii) involvement of the thalamus and superior parietal regions during the classification phase of each trial, and (iii) differential involvement of the caudate head during feedback. The effects of learning were then evaluated for both classification and feedback. Early in learning, subjects showed increased activation in the hippocampal regions during classification and activation in the heads of the caudate nuclei during the corresponding feedback phases. The findings suggest that early stages of prototype-distortion learning are characterized by networks previously associated with strategies of explicit memory and hypothesis testing. However as learning progresses the networks change. This finding suggests that the cognitive strategies also change during prototype-distortion learning. 相似文献
20.
Friendship patterns of 117 children with learning disabilities (LD) and 115 children without LD in Grades 4–8 were examined. In comparison with children without LD, boys with LD had fewer mutual friends, children with LD had more friends with learning problems and more younger friends, and children with LD in Grades 4–6 had less stable relationships. With regard to friendship quality, children with LD reported higher levels of conflict, lower levels of validation, and more problems with relationship repair than did children without LD. The findings were discussed in terms of factors that have been found to enhance friendship such as proximity and similarity, and the social skills difficulties that have been associated with learning disabilities. 相似文献