全文获取类型
收费全文 | 572篇 |
免费 | 58篇 |
国内免费 | 13篇 |
专业分类
643篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 2篇 |
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 22篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 19篇 |
2018年 | 21篇 |
2017年 | 37篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 26篇 |
2014年 | 24篇 |
2013年 | 132篇 |
2012年 | 33篇 |
2011年 | 22篇 |
2010年 | 22篇 |
2009年 | 30篇 |
2008年 | 26篇 |
2007年 | 26篇 |
2006年 | 23篇 |
2005年 | 25篇 |
2004年 | 5篇 |
2003年 | 8篇 |
2002年 | 16篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有643条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
541.
Tamsin K. Saxton Anthony C. Little Lisa M. DeBruine Benedict C. Jones S. Craig Roberts 《Personality and individual differences》2009,47(8):864-868
Exposure to a particular population of faces can increase ratings of the normality and attractiveness of similar-looking faces. Such exposure can also refine the perceived boundaries of that face population, such that other faces are more readily perceived as dissimilar. We predicted that relatively less exposure to opposite-sex faces, as experienced by children at single-sex compared with mixed-sex schools, would decrease ratings of the attractiveness of sexual dimorphism in opposite-sex faces (that is, boys at single-sex schools would show a decreased preference for feminised faces, and girls at single-sex schools would show a decreased preference for masculinised faces). Consistent with this prediction, girls at single-sex compared with mixed-sex schools demonstrated significantly stronger preferences for facial femininity in both male and female faces. Boys at single-sex compared with mixed-sex schools demonstrated marginally stronger preferences for facial masculinity in male faces, but did not differ in their ratings of female faces. These effects were attenuated among some single-sex school pupils by the presence of adolescent opposite-sex siblings. These data add to the evidence that long-term exposure to a particular face population can influence judgements of other faces, and contribute to our understanding of the factors leading to individual differences in face preferences. 相似文献
542.
大学生时间管理倾向对其社会适应能力的影响 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
该研究对180名大学生的时间管理倾向及其社会适应能力的关系进行探讨。结果发现,被试的时间管理倾向及其3个维度(时间价值感、时间监控观、时间效能感),以及社会适应能力在性别、学科等因素上没有显著差异;时间管理倾向高分组和低分组被试在社会适应能力上有极其显著的差异。将社会适应能力对时间管理倾向3个维度进行逐步回归分析,结果表明,时间效能感对社会适应能力有好的预见性。因此,开展卓有成效的时间管理训练可以作为提高大学生社会适应能力的一种方法。 相似文献
543.
IntroductionThe Highly Sensitive Person Scale based on the sensory-processing sensitivity is a self-assessment questionnaire consisting of 27 items. The scale is designed in order to identify individuals with high sensitivity.ObjectiveThe objective was to develop a French version of the scale. We tested its internal consistency and test-retest reliability on a French population sample. Another aim of this study was also to question the multidimensionality of the scale, for which several different models are suggested within the literature.MethodAfter translation and back-translation, a validation study was conducted on 814 adults. They were invited to complete an online questionnaire during the lockdown implemented due to COVID 19, between March 31st and May 11th (2020).ResultsThe internal reliability of the French version of HSPS was very good, with a Cronbach's alpha of .90, as was that of the factors. Correlations between factors were significant (p < .001). The intra-class correlation (ICC) for test-retest was .889 (0.874–0.903; 95% confidence interval). Factor analyses suggested a 4-factors structure, mixing the models found in the literature.ConclusionsThis study focused on a French adaptation of the HSPS scale. The results showed good psychometric qualities and stayed true to the original HSPS scale. The scale could be useful both to practitioners in their clinical practice and to researchers in fundamental research. 相似文献
544.
Robert Arp 《Journal for General Philosophy of Science》2007,38(1):19-30
Summary In the biological realm, a complete explanation of a trait seems to include an explanation in terms of function. It is natural
to ask of some trait, “What is its function?” or “What purpose in the organism does the particular trait serve?” or “What
is the goal of its activity?” There are several views concerning the appropriate definition of function for biological matters.
Two popular views of function with respect to living things are Cummins’ organizational account and the Griffiths/Godfrey-Smith
modern history account. Whereas Cummins argues that a trait functions so as to contribute to the general organization of some
organism’s present structure, Griffiths, and Godfrey-Smith argue that a trait functions because of its fitness with respect
to the organism’s recent evolutionary history. In this paper, I show how these accounts can be made compatible and compliment
one another. Given that structure, organization, operational flexibility, function, and evolutionary history are all factors
to be considered in an organism’s makeup, we should expect that the traits of an organism function the way they do because
such traits presently contribute to the overall organization of the organism (Cummins) as well as were selected for in the
organism’s species’ recent ancestry (Griffiths/Godfrey-Smith). 相似文献
545.
Body image disturbance is characteristic of eating disorders, and current treatments use body exposure to reduce bad body feelings. There is however little known about the cognitive effects of body exposure. In the present study, eye movement registration (electroculography) as a direct index of selective visual attention was used while eating symptomatic and normal control participants were exposed to digitalized pictures of their own body and control bodies. The data showed a decreased focus on their own 'beautiful' body parts in the high symptomatic participants, whereas inspection of their own 'ugly' body parts was given priority. In the normal control group a self-serving cognitive bias was found: they focused more on their own 'beautiful' body parts and less on their own 'ugly' body parts. When viewing other bodies the pattern was reversed: high symptom participants allocated their attention to the beautiful parts of other bodies, whereas normal controls concentrated on the ugly parts of the other bodies. From the present findings the hypothesis follows that a change in the processing of information might be needed for body exposure to be successful. 相似文献
546.
Decision making in the Ultimatum game requires the resolution of conflicts between economic self-interest and fairness intuitions. Since cognitive control processes play an important role in conflict resolution, the present study examined how control processes that are triggered by conflicts between fairness and self-interest in unfair offers affect subsequent decisions in the Ultimatum game. Our results revealed that more unfair offers were accepted following previously unfair, compared to previously fair offers. Interestingly, the magnitude of this conflict adaptation effect correlated with the individual subjects' focus on economic self-interest. We concluded that conflicts between fairness and self-interest trigger cognitive control processes, which reinforce the focus on the current task goal. 相似文献
547.
548.
This study examined the effects of post-cue interval and cognitive load on item-method directed forgetting. The results of Experiment 1 (free recall test) and Experiment 2 (cued recall test) showed that forget item retention increased as the post-cue interval increased. Moreover, increasing the cognitive load of participants by asking them to perform a secondary counting task did not impair, but rather facilitated, the intentional forgetting of the studied item under long post-cue interval conditions. These results and analyses of recall gains from the additional use of the independent cue suggest that the improved recall of forget items caused by an increase in the post-cue interval came from an automatic process, and that after receiving the forget cue, participants did not engage a suppression operation that was resource-demanded. The current findings suggest that forgetting is more effective when participants perform a secondary task after receiving the forget cue. 相似文献
549.
Solanto MV Gilbert SN Raj A Zhu J Pope-Boyd S Pope-Boyd S Stepak B Vail L Newcorn JH 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2007,35(5):729-744
The Predominantly Inattentive (PI) and Combined (CB) subtypes of AD/HD differ in cognitive tempo, age of onset, gender ratio, and comorbidity, yet a differentiating endophenotype has not been identified. The aim of this study was to test rigorously diagnosed PI, CB, and typical children on measures selected for their potential to reveal hypothesized differences between the subtypes in specific neurocognitive systems (anterior vs. posterior attentional systems) and processes (arousal vs. activation). Thirty-four CB and 26 PI children meeting full DSM-IV criteria for subtype both in school and at home, without confounding reading disability or emotional disorder, were enrolled along with 20 typically developing children. Neurocognitive functions measured included attention, inhibitory control, working memory, learning, and executive functions. Tasks included the Stroop, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Continuous Performance Test (CPT). Buschke Selective Reminding Test, ad the Tower of London (TOL), as well as instruments developed by Posner and Sternberg, and tasks assessing the impact on reaction time of [corrected] varying preparatory intervals and stimulus/response complexity. After co-varying for IQ, subtypes differed primarily on measures of impulsivity during tests of vigilance (CPT) and executive function (TOL), with the CB group showing greater impulsivity than both other groups. In addition, the PI group showed worse performance than CB and control groups on the WISC-III Processing Speed Index. Whether analyzed with or without an IQ co-variate, there was no support in the data for hypothesized differences between subtypes in functioning of the anterior vs. posterior attentional systems, nor in involvement of arousal vs. activation processes. The results indicate that the PI and CB subtypes are best differentiated by ratings, observations and tests of cognitive tempo and behavioral impulsivity. Neuropsychological methods have yet to identify critical neuropsychological [corrected] substrates of these differences. 相似文献
550.
Memory for feature binding comprises a key ingredient in coherent object representations. Previous studies have been equivocal about human capacity for objects in the visual working memory. To evaluate memory for feature binding, a type identification paradigm was devised and used with a multiple-object permanence tracking task. Using objects defined by shape and color, observers identified types of changes in feature combinations across an occlusion event, and the effects of object motion and number of switches were investigated. With only one switch, task performance was impaired even under stationary conditions, suggesting highly limited capacity of binding memory. Second switch improved performance only in the stationary condition, suggesting that object motion strongly disrupts feature binding. Further analyses and experiments suggest that improvement by the second switch reflects transition of binding memory by selective attention. 相似文献