全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3184篇 |
免费 | 216篇 |
国内免费 | 57篇 |
专业分类
3457篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 2篇 |
2024年 | 29篇 |
2023年 | 62篇 |
2022年 | 65篇 |
2021年 | 103篇 |
2020年 | 186篇 |
2019年 | 215篇 |
2018年 | 177篇 |
2017年 | 162篇 |
2016年 | 182篇 |
2015年 | 106篇 |
2014年 | 158篇 |
2013年 | 503篇 |
2012年 | 117篇 |
2011年 | 115篇 |
2010年 | 101篇 |
2009年 | 136篇 |
2008年 | 148篇 |
2007年 | 140篇 |
2006年 | 91篇 |
2005年 | 105篇 |
2004年 | 68篇 |
2003年 | 58篇 |
2002年 | 51篇 |
2001年 | 36篇 |
2000年 | 37篇 |
1999年 | 20篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 22篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 20篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 19篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 21篇 |
1980年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 15篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 14篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有3457条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
921.
922.
Stewart Candlish 《Frontiers of Philosophy in China》2016,11(1):35-53
This article is in six main sections.In the first three sections,some indication of how and why philosophers have differed in their response to the title question is given by describing Wittgenstein's encounters with Camap,and by examining Wittgenstein's commitment to clarity and argument in philosophy,illustrating this commitment by reference to his Philosophical Investigations discussion of the will.In the remaining three sections,Russell is taken as a paradigm example of a central kind of analytic philosopher.The answer to the title question is unfolded by sketching Wittgenstein's and Russell's treatments of a few philosophical topics and problems,focusing on theories and questions surrounding propositions,judgments,and their constituents,in particular Russell's multiple relation theory of judgment and the question of the unity of the proposition.This approach displays,and does not merely assert,Russell's deployment of (sometimes repeated variants of) technical solutions to philosophical problems and how that deployment contrasts with Wittgenstein's attempts to make such problems disappear. 相似文献
923.
近年来,面孔研究已渗透进领导力研究领域,并掀起了一股领导者面孔特征的研究热潮。目前,可操作化的领导者面孔特征主要包括:面孔年龄、面孔宽高比和面孔特质,这些面孔特征不仅会影响下属的态度和行为,还能提供客观的组织绩效信息。认知心理学的内隐领导理论、原型匹配理论和进化心理学的优良基因理论、本能释放理论是上述机制的重要依据。未来研究可探讨华人领导者的娃娃脸效应及权变因素。 相似文献
924.
Mozaz M Garaigordobil M Gonzalez Rothi LJ Anderson J Crucian GP Heilman KM 《Brain and cognition》2006,62(3):241-245
BACKGROUND: Apraxia is neurologically induced deficit in the ability perform purposeful skilled movements. One of the most common forms is ideomotor apraxia (IMA) where spatial and temporal production errors are most prevalent. IMA can be associated Alzheimer's disease (AD), even early in its course, but is often not identified possibly because the evaluation of IMA by inexperienced judges using performance tests is unreliable. The purpose of this study, therefore, is to learn if the Postural Knowledge Test (PKT), a praxis discrimination test that assesses knowledge of transitive (PKT-T subtest) and intransitive (PKT-I subtest) postures and does not require extensive training, is as sensitive and specific as the praxis performance tests. METHODS: We studied 15 subjects with probable AD as well as 18 age-matched controls by having them perform transitive and intransitive gestures to command and imitation, as well as having them discriminate between correct and incorrect transitive and intransitive postures. RESULTS: Overall on all tests, the control subjects performed better than those with AD. In addition all subjects had more trouble with transitive than intransitive gestures. Using a stepwise discriminative analysis, 81.8% of the subjects could be classified according to Group (94.4% of Controls, 66.7% of AD subjects). In this analysis, the PKT-T (transitive posture subtest) was the only measure that contributed to the discrimination of subjects. CONCLUSION: We found that having subjects select the correct transitive hand postures in this "booklet test" was more sensitive than grading their praxis performances even when using judges with extensive training. This suggests that this discrimination test might be an excellent means for diagnosing and screening patients for AD. The reason why recognition of transitive postures is relatively more difficult for our AD subjects is not known. Two possibilities are that the representations for intransitive movements are stronger than those for transitive movements, and hence, more resistant to degradation, or that intransitive acts are stored in parts of the brain not affected by AD. 相似文献
925.
Strong C 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》2006,27(5):433-453
The President’s Council on Bioethics has addressed the moral status of human preembryos in its reports on stem cell research and human therapeutic cloning. Although the Council has been criticized for being hand-picked to favor the right-to-life viewpoint concerning human preembryos, it has embraced the idea that the right-to-life position should be defended in secular terms. This is an important feature of the Council’s work, and it demonstrates a recognition of the need for genuine engagement between opposing sides in the debate over stem cell research. To promote this engagement, the Council has stated in secular terms several arguments for the personhood of human preembryos. This essay presents and critiques those arguments, and it concludes that they are unsuccessful. If the best arguments in support of the personhood of human preembryos have been presented by the Council, then there are no reasonable secular arguments in support of that view. 相似文献
926.
This article provides an overview of recent developments in solving the timing problem (discreteness vs. continuity) in cognitive neuroscience. Both theoretical and empirical studies have been considered, with an emphasis on the framework of operational architectonics (OA) of brain functioning (Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts in Brain Mind 2:291-29, 2001; Neurosci Biobehav Rev 28:827-836, 2005). This framework explores the temporal structure of information flow and interarea interactions within the network of functional neuronal populations by examining topographic sharp transition processes in the scalp EEG, on the millisecond scale. We conclude, based on the OA framework, that brain functioning is best conceptualized in terms of continuity-discreteness unity which is also the characteristic property of cognition. At the end we emphasize where one might productively proceed for the future research. 相似文献
927.
During cognitive processing, the various cortical areas, with specialized functions, supply for different tasks. In most cases
then, the information flows are processed in a parallel way by brain networks which work together integrating the single performances
for a common goal. Such a step is generally performed at higher processing levels in the associative areas. The frequency
range at which neuronal pools oscillate is generally wider than the one which is detectable by bold changes in fMRI studies.
A high time resolution technique like magnetoencephalography or electroencephalography is therefore required as well as new
data processing algorithms for detecting different coherent brain areas cooperating for one cognitive task. Our experiments
show that no algorithm for the inverse problem solution is immune from bias. We propose therefore, as a possible solution,
our software LOCANTO (LOcalization and Coherence ANalysis TOol). This new package features a set of tools for the detection
of coherent areas. For such a task, as a default, it employs the algorithm with best performances for the neural landscape
to be detected. If the neural landscape under attention involves more than two interacting areas the SLoreta algorithm is
used. Our study shows in fact that SLoreta performance is not biased when the correlation among multiple sources is high.
On the other hand, the Beamforming algorithm is more precise than SLoreta at localizing single or double sources but it gets
a relevant localization bias when the sources are more than three and are highly correlated. 相似文献
928.
The current study investigated the influence of presentation modality (live, video, and slide show) on children's memory, suggestibility, recognition, and metamemorial monitoring processes. A total of 270 children in three age groups (5- and 6-year-olds, 7- and 8-year-olds, and 9- and 10-year-olds) watched a magic show and were questioned about it 1 week later. The live show yielded more correct answers to nonleading questions, higher resistance to misleading questions, and better recognition memory than did the video condition, which in turn resulted in better performance than did the slide show. Although presentation modality raised the general level of memory performance, the effects were equally strong in all age groups and did not affect memory phenomena such as the size of the misinformation effect and confidence judgments. 相似文献
929.
Inviting speculation has been found to increase children's false recall. In this study, kindergartners and third graders saw a clown perform actions alone or in interaction with a child. Two weeks later, the speculation group recalled all actions and was asked to speculate on half the actions. The control group recalled all actions without speculating. Four weeks after the show, all children recalled all actions again. The speculation group gave more false answers to the speculated items than the control group. Surprisingly, older children tended to report as many if not more false responses than younger children, regardless of speculation. In the speculation group, there were fewer false answers for interactions than actions, but false answers did not differ across observation types in the control group. Finally, speculation did not affect free and cued recall differentially. 相似文献
930.
The present study investigates the production of regular and irregular verbs in the past tense and the comprehension of passive sentences by Greek-speaking PD patients, and compares their behavior to that of normal speakers. Although the two groups manifest large scale differences at all the above constructions, the behavior of PDs is not different at regular vs. irregular past tense formation neither did we obtain strong evidence that they do not comprehend passives, most importantly, they certainly do not perform at chance. On the basis of the above, we conclude that there are no indications for a clearly linguistic deficit of the PD group, hence, their difference with the control group should be attributed to other factors, such as the computational demands of the tasks. 相似文献