全文获取类型
收费全文 | 320篇 |
免费 | 13篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
336篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2023年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 16篇 |
2018年 | 14篇 |
2017年 | 11篇 |
2016年 | 9篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 7篇 |
2013年 | 34篇 |
2012年 | 8篇 |
2011年 | 12篇 |
2010年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 13篇 |
2007年 | 8篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 15篇 |
2003年 | 8篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 1篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有336条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
321.
Caleb R. Stanley Jordan Belisle Mark R. Dixon 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2018,51(2):352-359
This study evaluated the efficacy of three equivalence‐based instruction procedures on the acquisition of novel academic skills by 3 adolescents diagnosed with autism in a school setting. The skills targeted for instruction were related to topics in history, science, and mathematics, and were taught using different training structures from the PEAK‐E curriculum. All participants demonstrated mastery of the trained relations and the tested derived relations following all variants of equivalence‐based instruction. 相似文献
322.
Transfer of control from picture to text‐based activity schedules has been shown to occur following conditional discrimination training in children with autism. This study extended this research by evaluating if conditional discrimination training could promote transfer of control in an adult with Down syndrome. The participant was taught to select photographs and pictures of kitchen tools when provided with dictated names. Then, he completed a text‐based activity schedule, matched printed words to photographs, and orally named printed words without direct training. 相似文献
323.
Erica D. Lozy Sarah C. Holmes Jeanne M. Donaldson 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2020,53(3):1337-1353
Students who fail to acquire foundational literacy skills during preschool are more likely to read below grade level average in elementary school and are at a heightened risk for future school failure, poverty, early mortality, and crime. The purpose of this study was to compare the effects and maintenance of and preference for paired kinesthetic movements (KM) to a traditional drill (TD) procedure on letter–sound correspondence and word recognition with 6 preschool children. In 6 of 11 evaluations, participants mastered the KM set in substantially fewer intervention sessions than the TD set. In 5 of 11 evaluations, participants mastered the KM and TD sets with little differentiation between the number of intervention sessions. No participant mastered the control set. Maintenance data demonstrate a higher number of correct responses for the KM condition across all weeks. Preference varied across participants and was not always consistent with the more effective intervention. 相似文献
324.
Teaching procedures that facilitate the emergence of novel responses allow for increased efficiency, which is critical when providing early-intervention services to children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Three 5- and 6-year-old children diagnosed with ASD participated. In Experiment 1, we demonstrated functional control over the effects of teaching echoic rehearsals on the emergence of completing novel two-step instructions via joint control and obtained generalization and maintenance of the effects in an applied, nontraining context. In Experiment 2, we conducted an experimental analysis of joint control by disrupting rehearsal of the instruction and tacts of the objects in the instruction, which served as the sources of joint control. Our results support the efficacy of the procedures for establishing first-trial performance with novel instructions and indicate that joint control is responsible for the emergent performance produced by our teaching procedures. 相似文献
325.
《The Journal of Humanistic Counseling》2017,56(3):197-210
The authors conducted a survey of students to solicit recommendations for promoting optimal student engagement in online counseling courses. On the basis of consensually agreed‐upon themes derived from a qualitative content analysis (Moustakas, 1994 ), they discuss the implications for promoting optimal student engagement. 相似文献
326.
Justin B. Leaf Joseph H. Cihon Aditt Alcalay Erin Mitchell Donna Townley‐Cochran Kevin Miller Ronald Leaf Mitchell Taubman John McEachin 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2017,50(2):304-316
The present study evaluated the effects of instructive feedback embedded within a group discrete trial teaching to teach tact relations to nine children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder using a nonconcurrent multiple‐baseline design. Dependent variables included correct responses for: primary targets (directly taught), secondary targets (taught via instructive feedback), primary observational targets (directly taught to other members of the group), and secondary observational targets (taught via instructive feedback provided to other members of the group). Results showed that all nine participants reached the mastery criterion for the primary targets, as well as acquired the secondary and observational targets without direct teaching. Clinical implications and areas for future research are provided. 相似文献
327.
328.
Students may exhibit two forms of cognitive biases, belief and hindsight bias, in evaluating a scientific experiment. Counter to disagreement, they may only believe an outcome that agrees with their belief to be more predictable in hindsight than foresight. The focus of this research is on the relationship between these biases. Students were queried about their dichotomous beliefs (learned vs. genetic) about behavior for an animal experiment and then assigned randomly to a no‐outcome or genetic outcome condition. With agreement between students' belief and outcome, the findings revealed hindsight bias (foreseeability) supported by the outcomes for surprise, disappointment, ethics, and research evaluation. With disagreement, hindsight bias was trumped along with perceiving the experiment as being less ethical and scientifically sound. Regardless of the outcome, students seem to adhere to their beliefs. Hence, students may believe that the outcome is inconsequential because it is obvious or contrary to their beliefs. 相似文献
329.
Misconceptions and realities about teaching online 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Sieber JE 《Science and engineering ethics》2005,11(3):329-340
This article is intended to guide online course developers and teachers. A brief review of the literature on the misconceptions
of beginning online teachers reveals that most accept the notion that putting one’s lecture notes online produces effective
learning, or that technology will make education more convenient and cost-effective for all concerned. Effective online learning
requires a high level of responsibility for learning on the part of students and a reduction of the teacher-student power
differential. This, in turn, has major implications for faculty and course development, student selection criteria, the cost
of instruction, and the outcomes of education. Effective online teaching focuses on processes of learning rather than outcomes,
and is consistent with modern principles of learning that emphasize focusing on issues of high interest to learners, teaching
students to use skills of active and effective learning, providing prompt feedback, and enabling students to establish learning
goals and employ alternative paths to achieving those goals. Multiple ways of operationalizing these goals online are presented.
Tips are offered concerning selection of students who are capable of benefiting from the online experience, developing curriculum
that is user-friendly, using resources that stimulate good writing and critical thinking, and limiting class size to a manageable
number. 相似文献
330.
Ottmar V. Lipp Kimberley M. Mallan Marilia Libera Mingshen Tan 《Behaviour research and therapy》2010,48(3):203-209
The current research assessed the effects of verbal instruction on affective and expectancy learning during repeated contingency reversals (Experiment 1) and during extinction (Experiment 2) in a picture-picture paradigm. Affective and expectancy learning displayed contingency reversal and extinction, but changes were slower for affective learning. Instructions facilitated reversal and extinction of expectancy learning but did not impact on affective learning. These findings suggest a differential susceptibility of affective and expectancy learning to verbal instruction and question previous reports that verbal instructions can accelerate the extinction of non-prepared fear learning in humans. 相似文献