全文获取类型
收费全文 | 8699篇 |
免费 | 293篇 |
国内免费 | 46篇 |
专业分类
9038篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 31篇 |
2023年 | 59篇 |
2022年 | 58篇 |
2021年 | 88篇 |
2020年 | 148篇 |
2019年 | 144篇 |
2018年 | 163篇 |
2017年 | 160篇 |
2016年 | 142篇 |
2015年 | 99篇 |
2014年 | 113篇 |
2013年 | 375篇 |
2012年 | 122篇 |
2011年 | 86篇 |
2010年 | 80篇 |
2009年 | 122篇 |
2008年 | 117篇 |
2007年 | 131篇 |
2006年 | 117篇 |
2005年 | 96篇 |
2004年 | 70篇 |
2003年 | 73篇 |
2002年 | 56篇 |
2001年 | 51篇 |
2000年 | 41篇 |
1999年 | 49篇 |
1998年 | 31篇 |
1997年 | 19篇 |
1996年 | 32篇 |
1995年 | 23篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 24篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 20篇 |
1987年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 462篇 |
1984年 | 590篇 |
1983年 | 506篇 |
1982年 | 568篇 |
1981年 | 546篇 |
1980年 | 538篇 |
1979年 | 477篇 |
1978年 | 529篇 |
1977年 | 420篇 |
1976年 | 405篇 |
1975年 | 332篇 |
1974年 | 341篇 |
1973年 | 289篇 |
排序方式: 共有9038条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
871.
872.
873.
874.
Lane has argued that kantowitz and Knight incorrectly predicted an interaction between primary and secondary task difficulty for the variable-allocation model of attention. It is shown that such an interaction is indeed obtained if task-performance difficulty is defined in terms of capacity and only reasonable transformations of data are permitted. This interaction holds for any set of monotonic resource operating characteristics. 相似文献
875.
This research compared the retention of nouns, adjectives and verbs of sentences. A model of the structure underlying the retention of sentences and in which deep structure constituent propositions are hierarchically related, was used to make predictions concerning the retention of these word classes. The model was not supported by the data and it was determined that latencies to recognize the words of a previously presented sentence were the same for all classes of words. This result indicated that for recognition (but not necessarily for other memory tasks), each word of a sentence is seperately or equally accessible (as long as inter-word imagery is the same). However, a further experiment showed that, although word class is not a variable, the image-arousing capacity of a word is. Latencies and error rates were much lower for high-imagery words than for low-imagery ones. The findings on error rates in recognizing words were interesting and pointed to a different model where the meaning representations and syntatic functions of words have an active role in sentence retention. 相似文献
876.
877.
Robert F DeWinne Tom D Overton Lawrence J Schneider 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1978,12(2):140-144
Employing a larger sample and a more stringent statistical criterion, this study replicated an attempt by T. G. Grandy and R. F. Stahmann (Journal of Vocational Behavior, 1974, 5, 231–239.) to test Holland's hypothesis that parents' personality types foster similar offspring personality development. The relationships between paternal and offspring personality types were consonant with the findings of Grandy and Stahmann but the role of maternal personalities remained unclear. An explanation was offered that fathers' personality types carry more weight in the development of offspring types. 相似文献
878.
Personnel representatives (N = 52) were shown one of four videotaped job interviews in which the verbal content of the 16-min interview was identical, but the interviewee's nonverbal behavior was manipulated. A “low nonverbal” interviewee was defined by minimal eye contact, low energy level, lack of affect and voice modulation, and a lack of speech fluency. The “high nonverbal” interviewee demonstrated the opposite behavior on each of these components. The subjects were asked to rate the videotaped candidates on dimensions previously identified as critical in influencing a job interviewer's decisions. Nonverbal behavior was found to have a significant effect on almost every rating made by subjects in this study. After reviewing the entire 16-min interview, 23 of the 26 subjects who saw the “high nonverbal” candidate would have invited him/her for a second interview. All 26 of the subjects who saw the “low nonverbal” candidate would not have recommended a second interview. 相似文献
879.
Nondepressed human subjects were divided into seven groups. On a series of discrimination problems, a helplessness group received insoluble problems, a solvable group received contingent feedback, and a no treatment control group received no feedback. For two other groups insoluble problems were preceded by success feedback on a different task presented according to a fixed ratio (FR) or variable ratio (VR) schedule of reinforcement. Two control groups received either FR or VR schedules of success but were not examined on the discrimination problems. All groups were tested for escape/avoidance performance on a human shuttle box. Both FR and VR schedules produced an inoculation against learned helplessness; escape performance by the helplessness group was significantly worse than that of FR and VR inoculation groups. These latter groups performed similar to the solvable and three control groups. Significantly worse than that of FR and VR inoculation groups. These latter groups performed similar to the solvable and three control groups. Significantly fewer subjects in the VR inoculation group exhibited avoidance responses than their counterparts in the FR inoculation group. despite similar escape performance. The findings indicate that learned helplessness can be prevented in humans and suggest different sources of interference produced by unpredictable and uncontrollable events. 相似文献
880.
Keith J Holyoak 《Cognitive psychology》1978,10(2):203-243
A model of subjective magnitude comparisons is explored, which assumes that subjects compare symbolic stimulus magnitudes with respect to a reference point. The reference point may be established implicitly by the question (e.g., “Which is larger?” vs “Which is smaller?”) or be presented explicitly (e.g., “Choose the stimulus closer to X.”). The model was tested in five experiments in which subjects judged which of two comparison digits was closer to (or further from) a reference digit. Regression analyses in three experiments revealed that reaction time depended on the ratio of the distances from the comparison items to the reference point. The other two experiments provided evidence that subjects can strategically vary the processes by which they compare stimuli to a reference point. The results indicated that subjects can perform various types of “analog arithmetic” using either the linear number scale or a nonlinear scale of subjective digit magnitude. 相似文献