全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1065篇 |
免费 | 48篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
1121篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 9篇 |
2023年 | 20篇 |
2022年 | 17篇 |
2021年 | 34篇 |
2020年 | 51篇 |
2019年 | 65篇 |
2018年 | 65篇 |
2017年 | 65篇 |
2016年 | 64篇 |
2015年 | 62篇 |
2014年 | 48篇 |
2013年 | 122篇 |
2012年 | 28篇 |
2011年 | 44篇 |
2010年 | 23篇 |
2009年 | 65篇 |
2008年 | 56篇 |
2007年 | 56篇 |
2006年 | 26篇 |
2005年 | 21篇 |
2004年 | 26篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 15篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 10篇 |
排序方式: 共有1121条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
231.
A new method is introduced for assessing children's component selection—i.e., the disposition to attend to a single feature of multifaceted stimuli. Eight-year-old children were found to exercise component selection to a lesser degree than 4-year-olds; while children at both age levels attended primarily to one stimulus component (shape), the older children showed a moderate amount of attention to a secondary redundant feature (color) as well. However, a comparable age difference in attention deployment was not observed when a single stimulus dimension (shape) was “relevant” in two variant tasks. These results imply a developmentally increasing ability to distinguish between conditions in which attending to redundant stimulus information can and cannot be useful. That this ability undergoes little further development beyond age 8 was suggested in a second experiment with 8- and 12-year-olds in which the three tasks produced relatively similar developmental trends in performance. 相似文献
232.
In a series of studies, young children often failed to construct and remember inferred relationships which they were capable of understanding. A cued recall procedure was employed to assess the relative effectiveness of implicit and explicit word prompts for sentence memory. The implicit cues were much less effective than the explicit cues for 6–7 yr old children while the cue types did not differ for 11–12 yr olds. Instructions to imitate the actions described by the sentences eliminated the memory difference for the young children. Developmental improvement in spontaneous inferential operations and intentional plans to remember are mechanisms which appear to yield an elaborated memory representation and greater access routes through indirect and implied relationships. 相似文献
233.
The association dimension of the Picture Identification Test (PIT) was used to investigate the motivation concepts of male and female university students classified as Declared Homosexual, Sexually Uncertain, Clinical Control, and Normal Control. The t test was used to identify Need Association Dyads which discriminated (p < .01) groups. Greater than chance numbers of variables discriminated each of the pairs of groups within each sex except the Sexually Uncertain versus Clinical Control Male groups, the Declared Homosexual versus Clinical Control Female groups, and the Declared Homosexual versus Normal Control Female groups. PIT Need Association Dyads were discussed as a type of implicit personality theory assessment with emphasis on the beliefs subjects hold about the simultaneous expression of needs. 相似文献
234.
Male college students were given the opportunity to deliver aversive noise to a partner (confederate) contingent on the partner's “mistakes” in a learning task. Subjects were either not informed about a reward or told that they, their partner, or a charity would receive a monetary reward for speedy learning. Half of the subjects observed the confederate cheat, while half did not observe any deceptive behavior. The intensity of punitive behavior seemed to vary in accordance with predictions derived from equity theory. That is, cheating for a charity, a “good cause” resulted in less intense punishment than did cheating for selfish gain. On the other hand, mistakes, uncomplicated by cheating, which deprived a charity were punished more intensely than were mistakes whose only result accrued to the confederate himself. 相似文献
235.
Liangkang Ni 《Frontiers of Philosophy in China》2007,2(4):547-556
With the help of the natural history of “zero,” and the use of “zero” as a starting point, one may consider two types of metaphysics.
On the one hand, the epistemological metaphysics, based on the perceptual/rational dichotomy, is related to the zero as a
vacancy between numbers. On the other hand, the genetic metaphysics, based on the dichotomy of source-evolution (or origin
and derivate), has much to do with the zero as a number between negative and positive numbers. In this respect, zero represents
the horizon of metaphysics: we can forever approach it, but we cannot ultimately arrive at it. Though serving as the point
of convergence and divergence for all relationships, zero has no definable content of its own. Such is the essence of zero,
and of metaphysics as well.
Translated by Chen Zhengzhi from Tongji Daxue Xuebao (Shehui Kexue Ban) 同济大学学报 (社会科学版) (Tongji University Journal, Social Sciences Edition), 2006, (1): 1–6. 相似文献
236.
L. Guilbert L. Auzoult D. Gilibert L. Sovet G. Bosselut 《Psychologie du Travail et des Organisations》2019,25(2):127-139
The inter-professional national agreement of 19 June 2013 emphasizes the role of managers in promoting quality of work life. The latter aims jointly at employees’ health and company performance due to commitment. In the present study we tested the indirect effect of ethical leadership on affective commitment and psychological flourishing via satisfaction with work–family balance. Among employees, 221 filled out a questionnaire. The results indicate, on the one hand, the positive links between ethical leadership and affective commitment and psychological flourishing, and on the other hand the mediating role of satisfaction with work–family balance in these two relationships. We provide recommendations to improve quality of work life. 相似文献
237.
Leonard S. Newman Collette P. Eccleston Masanori Oikawa 《The Journal of social psychology》2017,157(2):152-164
A personal history of being the target of biased negative evaluation may lead individuals to habitually divert attention from negative feedback when it is possible to do so. Two studies tested for the first time the hypothesis that people belonging to a stigmatized group (Black students on a predominantly White campus) will, relative to non-stigmatized people, be more likely to engage in mnemic neglect—that is, they will reveal a greater tendency to insulate themselves from the effects of negative self-relevant feedback by means of motivated forgetting. The results of Study 1 supported that hypothesis. In Study 2, priming the concept of egalitarianism reduced the tendency of Black participants to engage in higher levels of mnemic neglect, consistent with the idea that the higher observed levels of mnemic neglect among stigmatized individuals derives from expecting biased, discriminatory responses from other people. 相似文献
238.
通过对认知风格既往文献的回顾和分析,从临床心理学的角度将认知风格定义为个体在解释生活事件时所偏爱的和习惯化的特征性认知方式,并确定了4个维度:过度概括化、选择性提取、个体化和灾难化。通过开放式问卷调查、参考国内外已有量表、自编项目及专家评定,形成27个项目的认知风格问卷初测版,并分别在大学生样本和非学生样本中进行测试,筛选出在两个样本中都符合心理测量学要求的16个项目,最终形成认知风格问卷正式版。对认知风格问卷正式版进行心理测量学指标考察,结果发现问卷的内部一致性信度、重测信度、结构效度、内容效度、聚合效 相似文献
239.
编制适合中国社会文化特点的成年人健康信念量表。根据健康信念对心理健康影响的大量研究结果,确定健康信念量表包括合理性和可控性两个维度的理论构想;通过访谈相关医务工作者以及在心理学专家中征集项目的方式形成初始问卷,并通过预测筛选,确定量表的最终项目。对天津市387名大学生、事业单位职员、公务员和农民工进行正式施测,检验量表的各项心理测量学指标。结果:健康信念量表包括两个维度,分别是可控性和合理性,量表具有良好的重测信度、内部一致信度、结构效度、内容效度以及校标关联效度。结论:健康信念量表具备令人满意的心理测量 相似文献
240.
近五年西方心理学重要期刊发表论文简评 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
摘要:以《心理学公报》、《心理学评论》和《心理学年度评论》三种心理学国际权威学术期刊从2005年到2009年发表的论文为基础,从研究领域、研究主题、引用率等方面对国际心理学的研究进行了文献计量分析。探讨了心理学发展趋势,指出基础/实验心理、社会心理和健康心理领域的研究仍然是心理学研究的主流,但研究方法、技术发生了根本改变;认知、学习与记忆、决策与判断、情绪健康等主题是心理学研究的热点,从而启发中国的心理学研究者应加强研究方法和统计的训练,关注研究的本土化,进行跨学科、多层次整合的研究以及拓展研究主题。 相似文献