排序方式: 共有340条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
122.
Sylvia Scribner 《Cognitive psychology》1974,6(4):475-494
Studies are reported among West African populations on the relationship between preferred mode of organization of categorizable material and use of organization in recall. Various child and adult groups differed in the types of organization they imposed on the material in a sorting task, with those exposed to school and modernizing influences showing a preference for taxonomic organization. All subjects, however, used their own form of organization to order their subsequent recall of the material, supporting the idea that organizing processes in recall are universal. 相似文献
123.
The expectation of cooperative future interaction (ECFI) encouraged especially high joint benefit (the total of the two bargainers' individual outcomes) when resistance to yielding was high, but especially low joint benefit when resistance to yielding was low. Process data suggest that this finding can be explained as follows: When resistance to yielding is high, ECFI encourages problem solving and reduced contentious behavior; when resistance to yielding is low, ECFI encourages a collapse of aspirations. The results support Filley's (1975) dualconcern model and cast doubt on Deutsch's (1973) blanket assertion that a cooperative process leads to constructive conflict resolution. 相似文献
124.
125.
126.
It was proposed that prediction hypotheses do not emerge full blown in the repertoire of the young child. Instead, the component units out of which predictions are consolidated (win-stay, lose-shift) are acquired independently and at different rates. The young child's behavior is at first dominated by response sets in which the win and lose components involve the same action (win-stay, lose-stay or win-shift, lose-shift). The components of response sets must be separated and coordinated with task-relevant information before appropriate components are consolidated to yield prediction hypotheses. A combination of learning-set and blank-trial procedures was used to evaluate this conception with preschoolers (mean CA = 4:1) and second graders (mean CA = 7:6). Results revealed that children of both age levels exhibited response patterns that corresponded to hypotheses in most of their probes (.89 to. 98). The striking finding was the relative weakness of win-stay on object cues at the outset of acquisition, particularly among the younger children. In contrast. lose-shift object was strong from the outset among children of both age levels. Once the two components (win-stay object, lose-shift object) were consolidated, the resulting scheme was durable, in that transfer to more complex problems was nearly perfect. Children who did not achieve criterion (in 70 problems) failed because they exhibited mostly position hypotheses and/or because the strength of win-stay object remained weak throughout all sessions. 相似文献
127.
Arthur J. Baroody 《Developmental Review》1983,3(2):225-230
In a review of the Chronometrie literature, M. H. Ashcraft (Developmental Review, 1982, 2, 213–236) concluded that the development of number fact efficiency is due to a shift from relying on procedural knowledge such as counting to relying on declarative knowledge (a stored network of facts). This model assumes that all procedural processes are slow or remain slow, which is probably not the case. An alternative account posits that the key change in number fact efficiency involves a shift from slow counting procedures to principled procedural knowledge. As rules, heuristics, and principles become more familiar and interconnected, their use, for example, in producing the number facts becomes more automatic. The use of such procedural knowledge would be cognitively more economical than storing individual facts in long-term memory. Finally, existing Chronometric data can readily be interpreted in terms of this alternative model. 相似文献
128.
129.
Gary S. Dell 《Cognitive Science》1985,9(1):3-23
Recent connectionist models of the perception and production of words make use of positive feedback from later to earlier levels of processing. This paper focuses on production and identifies several specific effects of phoneme-tomorpheme feedback. In addition, I argue that there is support for the use of this kind of feedback in production from experimental and naturalistic studies of slips of the tongue. 相似文献
130.