全文获取类型
收费全文 | 805篇 |
免费 | 75篇 |
国内免费 | 21篇 |
专业分类
901篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 4篇 |
2024年 | 11篇 |
2023年 | 14篇 |
2022年 | 22篇 |
2021年 | 28篇 |
2020年 | 33篇 |
2019年 | 45篇 |
2018年 | 49篇 |
2017年 | 40篇 |
2016年 | 37篇 |
2015年 | 39篇 |
2014年 | 44篇 |
2013年 | 140篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 24篇 |
2010年 | 16篇 |
2009年 | 29篇 |
2008年 | 43篇 |
2007年 | 31篇 |
2006年 | 34篇 |
2005年 | 22篇 |
2004年 | 22篇 |
2003年 | 21篇 |
2002年 | 9篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有901条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
White KG Pipe ME McLean AP Redman S 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1985,44(2):207-215
Distributions of reinforcers between two components of multiple variable-interval schedules were varied over a number of conditions. Sensitivity to reinforcement, measured by the exponent of the power function relating ratios of responses in the two components to ratios of reinforcers obtained in the components, did not differ between conditions with 15-s or 60-s component durations. The failure to demonstrate the “short-component effect,” where sensitivity is high for short components, was consistent with reanalysis of previous data. With 60-s components, sensitivity to reinforcement decreased systematically with time since component alternation, and was higher in the first 15-s subinterval of the 60-s component than for the component whose total duration was 15 s. Varying component duration and sampling behavior at different times since component transition may not be equivalent ways of examining the effects of average temporal distance between components. 相似文献
2.
Pigeons' choices between alternatives that provided different percentages of reinforcement in mixed schedules were studied using the concurrent-chains procedure. In Experiment 1, the alternatives were terminal-link schedules that were equal in delay and magnitude of reinforcement, but that provided different percentages of reinforcement, with one schedule providing, reinforcement twice as reliably as the other. All pigeons preferred the more reliable schedule, and their level of preference was not systematically affected by variation in the absolute percentage values, or in the magnitude of reinforcement. In Experiment 2, preference for a schedule providing 100% reinforcement over one providing 33% reinforcement increased systematically with increases in the duration of the terminal links. In contrast, preference decreased systematically with increases in the duration of the initial links. Experiment 3 examined choice with equal percentages of reinforcement but unequal delays to reinforcement. Preference for the shorter delay to reinforcement was not systematically affected by variation in the absolute percentage of reinforcement. The overall pattern of results supported predictions based on an extension of the delay-reduction hypothesis to choice procedures involving mixed schedules of percentage reinforcement. 相似文献
3.
Following substantial bleaching by "white" light, absolute threshold, relative spectral sensitivity and sensation of hue of monochromatic lights were measured at the central fovea during the cone-plateau period. The absolute-threshold level was found to increase and then decrease markedly, the relative spectral sensitivity remained invariant, while the sensation of hues of monochromatic lights from the long- and middle-wave regions of the spectrum changed toward hues of shorter wavelengths. 相似文献
4.
The residential school experience was devastating for many First Nation (Indian) people. The lingering effects of this era have been far reaching and remain painfully evident throughout Native communities. One of the more obvious areas directly affected has been parenting. This paper discusses current parenting difficulties of Native clients who were formerly in residential schools. A connection is made between the abuse experienced by these individuals and their parental struggles. Along with addressing various sociopolitical issues, a number of clinical interventions designed to promote the healing process of these victims and their families are provided and discussed. 相似文献
5.
R. J. Rodgers 《Aggressive behavior》1977,3(3):277-288
Two aspects of the amygdaloid complex (corticomedial and basolateral) were examined with reference to serotonergic inhibition of shock-induced aggression. Fighting was significantly depressed by serotonergic stimulation (5-HT, 10 μg bilateral) in the corticomedial amygdala while serotonergic blockade (methysergide, 5 μg bilateral) in this region increased levels of fighting. No consistent effects were obtained with serotonergic manipulation of the basolateral amygdala. Further investigation revealed that the state of serotonergic activity in medial amygdaloid sites was associated with concomitant alterations in the animals' sensitivity to footshock. Results are discussed in relation to a) a general inhibitory role of serotonin in behavioural mechanisms and b) a dopaminergic-serotonergic balance for behavioural arousal involving medial amygdaloid nuclei. 相似文献
6.
7.
Peter Muris Jorg Huijding Birgit Mayer Marit Langkamp Ela Reyhan Bunmi Olatunji 《Behavior Therapy》2012
The present study examined the psychometric properties of the Disgust Emotion Scale for Children (DES-C). Principal components analysis of the DES-C data revealed five factors reflecting disgust toward (a) rotting foods, (b) injection and blood, (c) odors, (d) mutilation and death, and (e) animals, which were largely in keeping with the intended subscales. The DES-C showed good reliability, excellent convergent validity (as established by correlations with an alternative self-report index of disgust), fairly good predictive validity (as assessed by correlations with measures of fear/anxiety and a behavioral index of disgust), and acceptable parent–child agreement (in particular with the mothers). Importantly, the DES-C proved to perform better on some psychometric indicators than an age-downward version of the Disgust Scale. These findings indicate that the DES-C should be regarded as the preferred scale for measuring disgust sensitivity and its role in the etiology and maintenance of anxiety problems in children. 相似文献
8.
《Philosophical Psychology》2012,25(6):727-738
Thomas Polger and Lawrence Shapiro (or P&S) have recently (2008) criticized “causal-mechanist” views of realization that dominate research in the philosophy of mind and metaphysics of science. P&S offer the internal criticism that any account of realization focusing upon property instances, as views of causal-mechanist realization routinely do, must lead to incoherence about multiple realization. P&S's argument highlights important issues about property instances that have recently been neglected, as well as raising a challenge to the standard approach to understanding the non-causal relations between properties and their instances in the sciences. In response, I clarify some important background issues about property instances and their relations to properties which show why P&S's main argument fails. In addition, I provide a second reason to doubt their argument by highlighting reasons to think that instances, as well as properties, are plausibly sometimes multiply realized in the sciences. 相似文献
9.
《Philosophical Psychology》2012,25(6):881-902
The Mind-Brain Identity Theory lived a short life as a respectable philosophical position in the late 1950s, until Hilary Putnam developed his famous argument on the multiple realizability of mental states. The argument was, and still is, taken as the definitive demonstration of the falsity of Identity Theory and the foundation on which contemporary functionalist computational cognitive science was to be grounded. In this paper, in the wake of some contemporary philosophers, we reopen the case for Identity Theory and offer a solution to the problem of multiple realizabilty. The solution is based on the necessity, at the time of establishing identity relations, of appealing to the notions of “homology” and “analogy” developed in the nineteenth century by Richard Owen. We also suggest that these notions are useful in order to correct certain shortcomings of some recent attempts at rebutting the Multiple Realizability argument. 相似文献
10.
《心理学报》2025,57(4)
越来越多的企业采用品牌拟人化沟通的方式进行品牌传播, 促进绿色消费倾向。本研究将基于习得可评估性理论, 探索品牌拟人化沟通对绿色消费倾向的影响机制及边界条件。本研究共包含4个实验。实验1表明品牌拟人化沟通能够有效促进绿色消费倾向, 构建了从品牌拟人化沟通到习得可评估性到品牌环保价值敏感性再到绿色消费倾向的完整因果链。实验2界定主效应的边界, 发现相比于普通品牌, 主效应在绿色品牌情境中更显著, 再次验证主效应的理论逻辑。实验3分析了消费者动机类型(利己/利他)对主效应的调节作用。结果表明,当消费者利他动机被激发时, 品牌拟人化沟通能够显著地促进绿色消费倾向, 当消费者利己动机被激发时, 品牌拟人化沟通不会影响绿色消费倾向。实验4验证了产品类型(搜索品/体验品)对主效应的调节作用。结果表明当产品类型为搜索品时,品牌拟人化沟通能够显著地促进绿色消费倾向, 当产品类型为体验品时, 品牌拟人化沟通不会影响绿色消费倾向。本研究拓展了品牌拟人化沟通以及绿色消费相关领域的研究。 相似文献