全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2638篇 |
免费 | 174篇 |
国内免费 | 45篇 |
专业分类
2857篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 4篇 |
2024年 | 38篇 |
2023年 | 45篇 |
2022年 | 51篇 |
2021年 | 71篇 |
2020年 | 124篇 |
2019年 | 146篇 |
2018年 | 144篇 |
2017年 | 161篇 |
2016年 | 130篇 |
2015年 | 131篇 |
2014年 | 109篇 |
2013年 | 328篇 |
2012年 | 111篇 |
2011年 | 90篇 |
2010年 | 55篇 |
2009年 | 116篇 |
2008年 | 157篇 |
2007年 | 162篇 |
2006年 | 109篇 |
2005年 | 82篇 |
2004年 | 49篇 |
2003年 | 50篇 |
2002年 | 48篇 |
2001年 | 26篇 |
2000年 | 23篇 |
1999年 | 19篇 |
1998年 | 24篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 17篇 |
1995年 | 22篇 |
1994年 | 17篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1978年 | 13篇 |
1977年 | 15篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有2857条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
941.
Ernst M. Falzeder John C. Burnham 《The International journal of psycho-analysis》2007,88(5):1223-1244
An eyewitness account provides evidence of a significant clandestine effort to neutralize the legitimacy and authority of psychoanalysis. In a letter, the witness confirms the existence of a perfectly staged concerted action among German psychiatrists against Freud's influence in 1913. Their congress in Breslau was meant to present the united front of German psychiatrists, who were going on record as being against psychoanalysis and, in that context, to give Eugen Bleuler, a leading psychiatrist, whose (however half-hearted) support for psychoanalysis had alarmed his colleagues, a public opportunity for back-pedalling. The letter shows that Freud and his allies were not the only ones who tried to manage an intellectual movement by using informal networks and 'behind the scenes' manoeuvring. 相似文献
942.
Fernando Riolo 《The International journal of psycho-analysis》2007,88(6):1375-1389
The author describes how Bion took Freud's conception of dreams as a form of thought and used it as the basis of his theory of transformations. Bion developed an expanded theory of ‘dream thought’, understood as a process of selection and transformation of sensory and emotional experiences. In this theory, the work of analysis is in turn conceived as a process not only of deciphering symbols, of revealing already existing unconscious meanings, but also of symbol production‐of a process for generating thoughts and conferring meaning on experiences that have never been conscious and never been repressed because they have never been ‘thought’. Analysis, in its specific operational sense, becomes a system of transformation whereby unconscious somatopsychic processes acquire the conditions for representability and become capable of translation into thoughts, words and interpretations. The rules of transformation applied by the patient in his representations and those applied by the analyst in his interpretations have the same importance for the analytic process as those described by Freud for the process of dreaming. The author discusses the broad categories of transformation adduced by Bion (rigid motion, projective, and in hallucinosis) and introduces some further distinctions within them. 相似文献
943.
CARMEN C. MION 《The International journal of psycho-analysis》2006,87(1):125-143
This work concerns a nine‐year analytical experience with a patient who presented as a main feature an apparent inability to experience and express feelings. Right from the beginning the author was confronted with the question of the viability of analytical work, considering transference or emotional involvement in the absence of perceptible establishment of an affective link on the part of the patient. The patient never missed a session, was usually very punctual and presented very rich material, but the accounts of his life, everything he was saying, hearing and observing was manifestly deprived of any emotional meaning for the patient and consequently deprived of sense for the analyst. If, at the beginning, the question was how to communicate with the patient, after some time it became a problem of how to enable the patient to communicate with himself. Confi rming an observation by Bion that the patient is the best colleague the analyst can have, the way forward was indicated by the patient himself. This article is a theoretical exercise based on this clinical experience, using concepts developed by Bion, Ferro and Winnicott. 相似文献
944.
Our goal in this paper is to examine the potential origins of children's understanding of morally relevant transgressions, with a particular focus on how children's perceptions of both proximal and distal unfairness might influence their social reasoning and behavior. A preliminary theoretical model is presented that addresses connections among aggressive children's social cognitive biases, their attachment histories, and their working models of societal justice and fairness. It is argued that difficulties in early parent–child interactions in combination with hostile larger social environments act to undermine emotional reciprocity, empathy, and concern for others in ways likely to promote proactive, uncaring forms of victimization and harm. Furthermore, it is proposed that given sufficiently toxic social experiences, some children will develop beliefs that life does not primarily revolve around caring or fairness, but around power and domination. Discussion focuses on the potential implications of these non-normative but coherent moral beliefs for theory and intervention. 相似文献
945.
Carlo Strenger 《International Journal of Applied Psychoanalytic Studies》2004,1(4):291-312
An extended case study of an analytic treatment examines the difficulty of identity formation for Sephardic Jews in Israel, on the background of negative stereotypization of this group by the Ashkenazi Elites during the first decades of Israel's existence. Zionism was based on the idea that a New Jew needed to be created, as the Old Diaspora Jew was seen as weak, dejected, and self‐deprecating. The hypothesis is advanced that the Old Jew remains a split‐off part of Israeli identity, which is experienced as threatening, and that psychic wounds often found in Sephardic Jews in Israel reflect the projection of disavowed aspects of the Old Jew by Ashkenazim onto Sephardim. The case example shows how this wider social reality influences transference–countertransference interactions in analytic treatment. The patient's transference love and the therapist's complex countertransference reaction are analyzed not only as individual issues but as reflecting Israeli history and its present social and political reality. Copyright © 2004 Whurr Publishers Ltd. 相似文献
946.
徐柏郁 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2008,29(9):24-25
子宫肌瘤是妇科最常见的良性肿瘤,发病率高、恶变率低,目前治疗方法较多,但各有利弊;众多子宫肌瘤患者从医心理复杂,甚至难以配合治疗,医生应针对不同部位、大小、症状的肌瘤及患者的不同年龄、生育需求和心理状态,制定出个体化、人性化治疗方案,以期取得患者机体和心理均满意的和谐疗效。 相似文献
947.
HERMAN T. TAVANI 《Metaphilosophy》2007,38(1):1-22
Abstract: This essay critically examines some classic philosophical and legal theories of privacy, organized into four categories: the nonintrusion, seclusion, limitation, and control theories of privacy. Although each theory includes one or more important insights regarding the concept of privacy, I argue that each falls short of providing an adequate account of privacy. I then examine and defend a theory of privacy that incorporates elements of the classic theories into one unified theory: the Restricted Access/Limited Control (RALC) theory of privacy. Using an example involving data‐mining technology on the Internet, I show how RALC can help us to frame an online privacy policy that is sufficiently comprehensive in scope to address a wide range of privacy concerns that arise in connection with computers and information technology. 相似文献
948.
Several studies have clearly shown that texting on a mobile phone increases crash risk (e.g. Dingus et al., 2006; Victor et al., 2014). However, the frequency of texting while driving still remains high (e.g. Vollrath, Huemer, Teller & Likhacheva, 2016). One reason may be that drivers are convinced that they are able to manage this dual task due to their competency in driving and texting. From a theoretical point of view, this may be true within limits – two well-learned, automatically processed tasks may require so few resources most of the time that interferences may not occur.In order to test this assumption, a study with a standardized driving simulator task (the lane change task, LCT; Mattes, 2003) was done with n = 40 drivers selected for their driving experience and tested for their texting abilities. The effect of driving experience (high vs. low) and texting competence (high vs. low) on driving performance was examined in single (driving only) and dual-task (driving and texting) conditions. Additionally, a subjective assessment of one’s task performance was obtained. Texting on the mobile phone significantly impaired driving performance. While driving experience did not have an influence, the deteriorating effect of texting was significantly less pronounced in highly competent phone users. Interestingly, this objective effect was not found in the subjective assessment. Drivers with a high texting competence felt as impaired as drivers with a low texting competence. This is in line with the finding that even in this simple driving task their performance was still significantly deteriorated as compared to driving, only. Thus, it seems that the reason why people text while driving is not that they are not aware of the performance loss. However, this awareness of the possible risk does not seem sufficient to prevent them from texting while driving. 相似文献
949.
《Revue Européene de Psychologie Appliquée》2021,71(2):100609
IntroductionAlzheimer's disease may modify moral judgment.ObjectiveIn two studies, we assessed the impact of dementia on blame and forgiveness. Study 1 compared the ways in which young adults, older adults, and older adults with dementia cognitively integrated two factors. Study 2 assessed the number of different factors that older adults with dementia were able to integrate during these moral judgments.MethodThe participants recorded their moral judgements in a blame task and in a forgiveness task. In study 1, the two questionnaires contained scenarios built from the combination of two factors. In study 2, the participants were confronted with the same tasks under three different conditions with scenarios that combined three, four or five factors.ResultsThe data from study 1 showed that the older adults with dementia did not combine the two factors in the same way as young adults did: the combination depended on the type of moral judgment. Study 2 revealed differences in moral judgment between older adults with dementia and adults without dementia in all tasks (i.e. with three, four or five factors combined).ConclusionDementia has an impact on moral judgments. Moral judgment among people with dementia is both task- and condition-dependant. 相似文献
950.
Across four studies (N = 4,381, Mage = 39.17 years, 56% female, 67% White, 12% Hispanic/Latinx, 11% Asian/Pacific Islander, and 9% Black/African American) the experience of heartbreak was considered within trait and narrative research paradigms. Participants indicated if they have ever experienced, and were currently experiencing, heartbreak. Personality traits, attachment styles, and narrative accounts of heartbreak were also assessed. Across all studies, 82% of participants indicated experiencing heartbreak and this event was largely unrelated to demographic and personality characteristics whereas 14% of participants reported being in a current state of heartbreak and these individuals were more likely to be single, neurotic, and insecurely attached. Lastly, storying heartbreak as leading to self‐improvement related positively with secure attachment. 相似文献