全文获取类型
收费全文 | 8138篇 |
免费 | 840篇 |
国内免费 | 612篇 |
专业分类
9590篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 39篇 |
2023年 | 192篇 |
2022年 | 171篇 |
2021年 | 185篇 |
2020年 | 422篇 |
2019年 | 408篇 |
2018年 | 413篇 |
2017年 | 425篇 |
2016年 | 358篇 |
2015年 | 258篇 |
2014年 | 376篇 |
2013年 | 1057篇 |
2012年 | 209篇 |
2011年 | 317篇 |
2010年 | 254篇 |
2009年 | 326篇 |
2008年 | 390篇 |
2007年 | 419篇 |
2006年 | 408篇 |
2005年 | 363篇 |
2004年 | 322篇 |
2003年 | 236篇 |
2002年 | 211篇 |
2001年 | 138篇 |
2000年 | 112篇 |
1999年 | 124篇 |
1998年 | 94篇 |
1997年 | 79篇 |
1996年 | 60篇 |
1995年 | 62篇 |
1994年 | 43篇 |
1993年 | 44篇 |
1992年 | 38篇 |
1991年 | 18篇 |
1990年 | 21篇 |
1989年 | 28篇 |
1987年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 134篇 |
1984年 | 127篇 |
1983年 | 105篇 |
1982年 | 87篇 |
1981年 | 66篇 |
1980年 | 73篇 |
1979年 | 67篇 |
1978年 | 94篇 |
1977年 | 56篇 |
1976年 | 42篇 |
1975年 | 31篇 |
1974年 | 24篇 |
1973年 | 28篇 |
排序方式: 共有9590条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Right CVAs, mild left CVAs, and non-brain-damaged adults received the Token Test under three conditions: quiet, white noise, and speech babble. Left CVAs performed most poorly of all groups in all conditions experiencing difficulty with subtests III, IV, and V. Right CVAs were inferior to controls on each of these three subtests in the speech babble condition only. Thus, right CVA difficulty with both memory and syntactic tasks was demonstrated in speech babble. These results are explained by the hypothesis that both hemispheres are necessary to process auditory information under complex listening conditions. Total brain capacity of right CVA patients may be overloaded if either memory load or linguistic complexity is increased in the presence of certain types of listening competition. 相似文献
92.
93.
94.
Individual and organizational correlates of employees who refuse seniority-based promotions in union lines of progression are examined. The sample consisted of 313 production employees of a large manufacturing complex. Correlational and discriminant analyses revealed that age, being female, and number of employees in the department related positively to promotion refusal, while years of education and pay showed a negative relationship. Of these variables, years of education was the strongest predictor. Potential explanations for the results relate to stress avoidance, perceived inabilities, and equity theory. Areas for future research are suggested. 相似文献
95.
96.
An Ames (1951, Psychological Monographs, 65(1, Whole No. 324)) static trapezoidal window, under monocular view, was used to test young infants' responsiveness to pictorial depth. When adults view this display monocularly with the smaller side of the window rotated toward them, they report that the orientation of the display becomes ambiguous: When the head is moved, the window may appear to be in the fronto-parallel plane or either side may appear closer. The 7-month-olds we tested appeared to experience a similar ambiguity; they reached to the near side of the rotated trapezoidal window with significantly less consistency or directedness than infants in a control group tested with a rotated object that lacked pictorial depth information. When 5-month-olds were tested, however, they reached with equal consistency to the closer side of the trapezoidal window and of the control display, apparently uninfluenced by the pictorial depth information available in the trapezoidal window. Thus, sensitivity to the pictorial information for depth that is present in the trapezoidal window appears to develop after the age of 22 weeks. 相似文献
97.
98.
The present study tested the hypothesis that individuals with the Type A coronary prone behavior pattern focus their attention more on important tasks, and less on trivial tasks than do Type B's. The study used a paradigm in which some subjects worked on math problems (important task) and rated intervening mood items (trivial task), whereas others rated mood items they were to memorize (important task) and worked on intervening math problems (trivial task). All subjects were later tested on recall of mood items. Results indicated that Type A's performed better on math problems and recalled more fatigue-related mood items when the task was important than when it was trivial; for Type B subjects, there was no difference in the number of math problems solved correctly or number of mood items recalled between important and trivial task conditions. Moreover, Type A's reported greater fatigue when mood recall was the important task than when math was the important task, whereas fatigue ratings of Type B's did not differ across these conditions. The findings suggest that Type A's suppress their attention to fatigue-related symptoms only when symptoms are not the objects of focus. 相似文献
99.
Meryl J. Wall C. Woodruff Starkweather Helen S. Cairns 《Journal of Fluency Disorders》1981,6(4):283-298
This study examined the location and rate of stuttering in the spontaneous speech of nine young stutterers (4.0–6.6 yr) in relation to certain aspects of syntax. It was designed to answer the following questions: Is there more stuttering at clause onset (boundary) than at other points in the clause? Does the type of clause influence rate of stuttering? Is there more stuttering on complex than on simple sentences? The syntactic analysis defined 10 clause types (seven pertaining to complex sentences and three pertaining to simple sentences); clause boundaries (taking into account “filler” words occuring in such positions); and hierarchical constituent structure within sentences. Stuttering was found with significantly greater frequency at clause boundaries than at internal (nonboundary) positions of clauses. This was apparent for nine of the 10 clause types, the exception being complement clauses. The word “and” at the beginning of simple and complex sentences provoked a high rate of stuttering. The hierarchical structure within the clause did not influence the location of stuttering, and sentence complexity was not a factor in the rate of stuttering. 相似文献
100.
Robert L. Hale 《Journal of School Psychology》1981,19(3):274-278
This study investigated the efficacy of predicting academic achievement as measured by the Wide Range Achievement Test using the factor scores of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised as potential predictors. Both instruments were administered to 200 school children referred for psychological evaluations. Factor scores for the WISC-R were computed according to equations provided by Gutkin (1978). These factor scores then were regressed in a stepwise manner on the Reading, Spelling, and Arithmetic standard scores from the Wide Range Achievement Test. Stepwise regressions also were performed using the traditional Verbal and Performance IQs as the potential predictors. A comparison was made between the results obtained by use of the factors or the more common scores. The results indicated that the Freedom From Distractibility factor score significantly aided in the prediction of Reading and Spelling achievement and was of primary importance in predicting Arithmetic achievement. When the stepwise results using the different scores were contrasted, the factor scores accounted for appreaciably more variance in Arithmetic than did the traditional Verbal and Performance IQs. Derived regression equations are reported along with a discussion of the interpretability of the Freedom From Distractibility factor. 相似文献