全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1244篇 |
免费 | 101篇 |
国内免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
1372篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 2篇 |
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 21篇 |
2022年 | 25篇 |
2021年 | 21篇 |
2020年 | 53篇 |
2019年 | 68篇 |
2018年 | 62篇 |
2017年 | 64篇 |
2016年 | 66篇 |
2015年 | 48篇 |
2014年 | 49篇 |
2013年 | 188篇 |
2012年 | 37篇 |
2011年 | 49篇 |
2010年 | 25篇 |
2009年 | 63篇 |
2008年 | 54篇 |
2007年 | 70篇 |
2006年 | 68篇 |
2005年 | 58篇 |
2004年 | 42篇 |
2003年 | 49篇 |
2002年 | 30篇 |
2001年 | 23篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 20篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有1372条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
871.
Rita Cascle 《Studies in Philosophy and Education》2004,23(5):393-408
The following contribution expands the current discussion on the status and function of the history of education at the methodological, or epistemological, level by introducing the perspective of a history of educational knowledge. This opens up a theoretical option for educational historiography that avoids the identification of history of education with history of a discipline, or the institutionalized knowledge of education and teaching. As a consequence, some history of education topoi – as the history of a discipline – can be called into question and practiced historically rather than educationally or morally. 相似文献
872.
青少年元记忆能力发展的认知研究 总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7
元记忆是个体所具有的与自己的记忆活动有关的信念及监控系统 ,其发展研究包括元记忆知识的习得与发展、元记忆监控能力的发展等。 2 0年来的研究表明 :青少年元记忆能力的发展具有年龄阶段性和关键期 ,年龄和教育训练是其两个重要影响因素。但该领域研究还存在理论概括性不高、被试代表性不强和比较缺乏多因素实验研究等问题。其未来的研究中宜采用多因素实验设计 ,不断提高研究的系统性和生态学效度 相似文献
873.
Rachel Waters Janet Morgan Chris Kemp-Philp 《British Journal of Guidance & Counselling》2018,46(2):160-172
This interpretivist community participatory study explores carers’ beliefs about the potential usefulness of counselling in relation to the caring role. Twenty semi-structured interviews with carers were transcribed and analysed thematically. All participants thought counselling could potentially be helpful to carers, but their ideas about the ways in which it would help varied according to whether or not they had personal experience of counselling. Only carers with counselling experience had an awareness of counselling as focussing on changing feelings, thoughts, attitudes and behaviours. The implications of the findings for the provision of counselling services are discussed. 相似文献
874.
Janne Huovila 《Science as culture》2018,27(3):375-397
Individualisation of healthy eating is a broad tendency in Western culture and is becoming a popular ideal in nutrition science. Public perception of dietary knowledge is central to this individualisation and many experts now aim to present dietary knowledge in a way which relates to individual experience, as well as a casuistic style in which particular cases are discussed in relation to general, universal principles. Analysis of casuistic narrative styles in the public communication about healthy eating by experts makes explicit their flexible use of both particular cases and universal claims, in which clinical and personal cases are narratively employed by the experts to invite readers to personally consider dietary knowledge. Such casuistic narration by experts relates human agency and particular bodies to universal concepts in a way that has generative and critical functions with respect to dietary knowledge and understanding. The public articulation of clinical narratives by experts make salient the intersubjective emergence and accumulation of dietary knowledge between expert reasoning and lay understanding. Personal narratives have an essential role in presenting casuistic reasoning through experts’ personal lives and making their own bodies relevant to public communication about healthy eating. This emphasises the experts’ personal agency in relationship to common norms and general knowledge. Furthermore, both clinical and personal narratives are used by experts in their assessment of the shortcomings in public health messages. Experts’ casuistic narration thus does not merely construct credibility but has epistemic functions in constructing dietary knowledge tangible with respect to actual eating practices. 相似文献
875.
Winston R. Sieck Edgar C. Merkle Trisha Van Zandt 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》2007
The ASC model of choice and confidence in general knowledge proposes that respondents first Assess the familiarity of presented options, and then use the high-familiarity option as a retrieval cue to Search memory for the purposes of Constructing an explanation about why that high-familiarity option is true. The ASC process implies that overconfidence results in part from a tendency to fixate on the high-familiarity option, to the neglect of the other option. If this implication is true, then judgment tasks requiring respondents to evaluate each option independently should result in reduced overconfidence as compared with standard judgment tasks. Two experiments tested this implication, and found that confidence and overconfidence were reduced when respondents evaluated options independently. The findings support the proposal that option fixation contributes to overconfidence, and also clarify the limitations of random error explanations of overconfidence. 相似文献
876.
Taking a panoramic view on the history of modern philosophy, we can learn that political philosophy, a new arena for modern
philosophy, has become an important field in philosophical studies since the later half of the 20th century. As far as the
problem domain of political philosophy is concerned, political philosophy is only a special form of philosophy. The revival
of political philosophy, however, indicates that philosophical inspection of political matters has regained legitimacy, and
also means the restaging of philosophy as a knowledge type at modern times. In one sense, we can view the newly-revived political
philosophy as typical modern philosophy, because its problem domain, its unique angle of looking into the life world and its
ideal concern about the actual world make it one of the best ways in which we can reflect the existence of mankind in modern
times.
__________
Translated from Zhexue Yanjiu 哲学研究 (Philosophical Studies), 2005 (6) by Teng Jianhui 相似文献
877.
Michael Barber 《Human Studies》2006,29(2):141-157
Frank Welz’s Kritik der Lebenswelt undertakes a sociology of knowledge criticism of the work of Edmund Husserl and Alfred Schutz that construes them as developing absolutist, egological systems opposed to the “processual” worldview prominent since the modern rise of natural science. Welz, though, misunderstands the work of Schutz and Husserl and neglects how their focus on consciousness and eidetic features pertains to the kind of reflection that one must undertake if one would avoid succumbing to absolutism, that uncovers the presuppositions of the processual worldview itself, and that secures a domain distinctive of philosophy over against sociology. Finally, Welz’s charge that Schutz favors a Neo-Kantian social scientific methodology contradictory to his phenomenology neglects the levels of Schutz’s discourse and ignores how the Weberian ideal-typical approach can be subsumed within phenomenology. 相似文献
878.
Joshua Habgood-Coote 《Australasian journal of philosophy》2019,97(1):86-104
The debate about the nature of knowledge-how is standardly thought to be divided between intellectualist views, which take knowledge-how to be a kind of propositional knowledge, and anti-intellectualist views, which take knowledge-how to be a kind of ability. In this paper, I explore a compromise position—the interrogative capacity view—which claims that knowing how to do something is a certain kind of ability to generate answers to the question of how to do it. This view combines the intellectualist thesis that knowledge-how is a relation to a set of propositions with the anti-intellectualist thesis that knowledge-how is a kind of ability. I argue that this view combines the positive features of both intellectualism and anti-intellectualism. 相似文献
879.
Kimberly E. Marble 《The Journal of genetic psychology》2019,180(1):17-30
The authors investigated children’s use of cultural status (i.e., foreign vs. American) and learning method to evaluate informants’ expertise in novel cultural practices. Ninety-six 6- to 9-year-olds heard about a foreign informant (i.e., member of an unfamiliar out-group) and an American informant (i.e., member of the participant’s in-group) who each learned about a novel cultural practice differently (i.e., from a person vs. from a book). Participants decided which informant executed the cultural practice better, which informant they would prefer to learn from, and which learning method they would want to use themselves (i.e., learning method preference). Overall, participants endorsed foreign informants over American informants and foreign informants who learned from a person were generally viewed as the preferred option for imparting information in this context. These findings suggest that during the transition to middle childhood, learning context is an important influence on children’s evaluations of cultural identity and learning methods. 相似文献
880.
Mark T. Nelson 《Argumentation》2003,17(1):35-42
Why have so many philosophers agonised over the possibility of valid arguments from factual premises to moral conclusions? I suggest that they have done so, because of worries over a sceptical argument that has as one of its premises, `All moral knowledge must be non-inferential, or, if inferential, based on valid arguments or strong inductive arguments from factual premises'. I argue that this premise is false. 相似文献