全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2468篇 |
免费 | 316篇 |
国内免费 | 96篇 |
专业分类
2880篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 11篇 |
2024年 | 32篇 |
2023年 | 57篇 |
2022年 | 65篇 |
2021年 | 77篇 |
2020年 | 119篇 |
2019年 | 119篇 |
2018年 | 104篇 |
2017年 | 117篇 |
2016年 | 88篇 |
2015年 | 84篇 |
2014年 | 94篇 |
2013年 | 322篇 |
2012年 | 76篇 |
2011年 | 78篇 |
2010年 | 69篇 |
2009年 | 68篇 |
2008年 | 79篇 |
2007年 | 89篇 |
2006年 | 112篇 |
2005年 | 101篇 |
2004年 | 81篇 |
2003年 | 84篇 |
2002年 | 76篇 |
2001年 | 53篇 |
2000年 | 66篇 |
1999年 | 50篇 |
1998年 | 29篇 |
1997年 | 31篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 29篇 |
1994年 | 29篇 |
1993年 | 26篇 |
1992年 | 34篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 14篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 14篇 |
1985年 | 28篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 15篇 |
1982年 | 19篇 |
1981年 | 18篇 |
1980年 | 19篇 |
1979年 | 22篇 |
1978年 | 18篇 |
1977年 | 33篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有2880条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
201.
弱智儿童家长的心理压力及相关因素研究 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
对200名弱智儿童家长的心理压力及相关因素进行研究,总的来说,弱智儿童的父、母亲之间在心理压力方面没有明显的差异;不同年级儿童的家长在心理压力方面存在明显差异。家长的心理压力与其对事件的应付方式、生活质量以及弱智儿童的行为表现之间有密切关系。 相似文献
202.
亲子教育缺失与"留守儿童"人格、学绩及行为问题 总被引:98,自引:2,他引:98
以“留守儿童”为被试,考察其家庭环境因素、人格、学绩、行为问题之间的关系,发现“留守儿童”处在相对不良的家庭环境中;不良的家庭环境因素与不良人格特质、行为问题、学业不良高相关,且后三者密切关联,相互加剧。讨论指出:对“留守儿童”问题的干预不仅要进行行为矫治和心理辅导,而且要整合社区、学校、家庭的力量,改善其家庭环境和心理氛围,为“留守儿童”建构有效的支持系统。 相似文献
203.
我国3~6岁儿童个性类型及发展特点的研究 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
该文在以往中国儿童个性结构研究基础上,以问卷调查(n=927)方式,将层次聚类和迭代聚类两种方法结合起来,同时采用理论分析等方法最终确定我国3~6岁儿童的个性类型包括认可型、矛盾型、拒绝型及中间型四类。其中认可型属于积极个性类型,矛盾型和拒绝型属于消极个性类型。研究表明,中班到大班阶段是幼儿个性发展相对重要的时期。男女幼儿在多个个性维度及大部分具体特质方面存在着明显的性别差异。 相似文献
204.
小学儿童空间物体位置编码(Ⅰ) 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
采用找物范式研究小学7、9、11岁儿童利用点与点之间的关系对目标物位置进行编码的认知发展。结果表明:两点距离编码和三点共线编码认知成绩的年龄效应显著,9岁组和11岁组儿童的认知成绩显著好于7岁组儿童的认知成绩。两个不同实验任务对儿童认知成绩的影响不同,7岁组儿童两点距离编码认知成绩显著好于三点共线编码认知成绩,9岁组和11岁组两个任务的认知成绩差异不显著。提供编码线索后,认知成绩有了较大的改善。 相似文献
205.
3.5~5.5岁学前儿童年龄认知发展的研究 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
该实验采用三个问题作业(“年龄比较”、“出生年龄”、“年龄变化”)及一个“图示作业”,调查了60名3.5~5.5岁儿童年龄认知的发生、发展情况,结果表明:3.5岁儿童处于年龄认知萌芽状态,4.5岁儿童年龄认知开始发生,5.5岁儿童的年龄认知已发展到较高水平。儿童年龄认知变化可用变化分割模型(简称CS模型)来解释。 相似文献
206.
Jana Ptáčková Silvie Lišková Aleš Kuběna Daniel Frynta 《European Journal of Developmental Psychology》2017,14(1):16-31
This study explores aesthetic preferences towards boa and python species among pre-school children and adults, and compares the ratings between the two groups. A set of snake photographs was presented (56 species) to children and adult respondents. The respondents were asked to select the five most preferred and five least preferred species. The children’s agreement on which species were ‘beautiful’ and which were ‘ugly’ was statistically significant and a positive relationship between the mean ranks provided by children and adults was also found (r = .54, P < .001). Children preferred species with thick necks and inconspicuous heads, usually small species, harmless to children. Large pythons probably posed more danger to children than they did to adults during human evolution in African savannas and therefore we hypothesize that young children’s aesthetic preferences were shaped by natural selection through interactions with dangerous snakes. 相似文献
207.
Kimberly?A.?SchreckEmail author James?A.?Mulick Johannes?Rojahn 《Journal of child and family studies》2005,14(1):101-109
Wide discrepancies exist concerning the reported occurrence of sleep problems for elementary school children. We describe parental perception of sleep problem behavior in elementary school aged children 5–12 years. Approximately 4% of parents reported their children experienced significant global sleep problems serious enough to adversely affect family functioning. Significant specific sleep problems reported by parents occurred for approximately 9–14% of the children in the sample. These ranges were in the low end of the ranges of sleep problems reported in previous literature for this age group. 相似文献
208.
Sixty-two first and second grade students (28 boys, 34 girls) were exposed to one of three commercial videotapes in which either all-boys (traditional condition) or all-girls (nontraditional) were playing with a toy. Participants in the control condition were exposed to nontoy commercials. After exposure to one of the conditions participants performed a toy sort where they were asked if six toys, including the two manipulated toys, were for boys, girls, or both boys and girls. Participants in the nontraditional condition were more likely to report that the manipulated toys were for both boys and girls than were participants in the traditional condition, who were more likely to report that the manipulated toys were for boys. This effect was stronger for boys than for girls. 相似文献
209.
To test theoretical predictions about the role of meaning connections in false memory, the effects of semantic cues and list repetition on children's false memories were evaluated across early childhood to mid-adolescence using the Deese–Roediger–McDermott (DRM) paradigm. True recall and false recall increased from 7 to 13 years. Study list repetition increased true recall of list words with the magnitude of this effect largest in the older children and adolescents. Repetition reduced false recall of critical lures in all age groups. Consistent with theoretical predictions, false recall of critical lures increased when children were informed of the thematic gist of each list prior to study. 相似文献
210.
Paul J. Carter Brendan Hore Leona McGarrigle Manon Edwards Gavin Doeg Rachel Oakes 《The journal of positive psychology》2018,13(2):110-121
Positive psychology interventions (PPIs) are effective in increasing well-being across the population. Whilst educators are recognising the importance of well-being in the classroom and of its long-term impact on life trajectory, the transformative potential of PPIs in educational settings is yet to be fully realised. This study investigates, for the first time, the effects of a PPI in school children by means of a daily dairy. Self-report questionnaires were used to measure well-being in school children aged 8–11 years. Across two studies, children kept a positive events diary, recording three experiences every day for a week. The intervention led to an increase in happiness and a decrease in depressive symptoms immediately following the intervention and at a three-month follow-up. Children who had unhappier baseline scores benefitted more from the intervention. This study demonstrates significant scope, in school settings, for targeted light-touch interventions to promote well-being in those with the greatest need. 相似文献