全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1248篇 |
免费 | 105篇 |
国内免费 | 33篇 |
专业分类
1386篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 20篇 |
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 17篇 |
2021年 | 24篇 |
2020年 | 46篇 |
2019年 | 60篇 |
2018年 | 41篇 |
2017年 | 54篇 |
2016年 | 62篇 |
2015年 | 43篇 |
2014年 | 40篇 |
2013年 | 147篇 |
2012年 | 45篇 |
2011年 | 54篇 |
2010年 | 30篇 |
2009年 | 48篇 |
2008年 | 45篇 |
2007年 | 58篇 |
2006年 | 67篇 |
2005年 | 40篇 |
2004年 | 43篇 |
2003年 | 28篇 |
2002年 | 23篇 |
2001年 | 40篇 |
2000年 | 26篇 |
1999年 | 18篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 19篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 8篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 15篇 |
1977年 | 10篇 |
1976年 | 10篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有1386条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
Norma L. Day-Vines;Jeremy Zhang;Fallon Cluxton-Keller;Dantavious Hicks;Connie Jones;Rufus Tony Spann;Dipika Daga;Courtney Agorsor; 《Journal of multicultural counseling and development》2024,52(3):173-188
The current study evaluated the validity of the broaching attitudes and behavior survey-clients (BABS-C), a self-report measure that examined minoritized clients’ perceptions of their counselors’ ability to discuss issues of race, ethnicity, and culture during treatment. A series of confirmatory factor analyses suggested that the BABS-C framework and its subconstructs achieved excellent model fit and stable factor structures. The measurement invariance was examined with seven covariates using multiple indicators and multiple cause methods. Implications for counseling research and practice are discussed. 相似文献
152.
Sebastian Olschewski Pavel Sirotkin Jörg Rieskamp 《The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology》2022,75(2):252-292
A standard approach to distinguishing people’s risk preferences is to estimate a random utility model using a power utility function to characterize the preferences and a logit function to capture choice consistency. We demonstrate that with often-used choice situations, this model suffers from empirical underidentification, meaning that parameters cannot be estimated precisely. With simulations of estimation accuracy and Kullback–Leibler divergence measures we examined factors that potentially mitigate this problem. First, using a choice set that guarantees a switch in the utility order between two risky gambles in the range of plausible values leads to higher estimation accuracy than randomly created choice sets or the purpose-built choice sets common in the literature. Second, parameter estimates are regularly correlated, which contributes to empirical underidentification. Examining standardizations of the utility scale, we show that they mitigate this correlation and additionally improve the estimation accuracy for choice consistency. Yet, they can have detrimental effects on the estimation accuracy of risk preference. Finally, we also show how repeated versus distinct choice sets and an increase in observations affect estimation accuracy. Together, these results should help researchers make informed design choices to estimate parameters in the random utility model more precisely. 相似文献
153.
David Messer Jennifer Kearvell-White Henrik Danielsson Dorothy Faulkner Lucy Henry Paul Ibbotson 《The British journal of developmental psychology》2022,40(3):453-470
The structure and development of executive functioning (EF) have been intensively studied in typically developing populations, with little attention given to those with Special Educational Needs (SEN). This study addresses this by comparing the EF structure of 132 adolescents (11–14 years-old) with SEN and 138 adolescents not requiring additional support (Non-SEN peers). Participants completed verbal and non-verbal assessments of key components of EF: inhibition, working memory and switching. Confirmatory Factor Analysis on each group tested one-, two- and three-factor models of EF. In both groups, there was statistical support for the fit of one- and two-factor models with no model being clearly better than the others; there was little support for three-factor models. Parsimony suggests that the one-factor model best represents the structure of EF. In light of our results, the implications for the nature of EF in early adolescence in both SEN and Non-SEN groups are discussed. 相似文献
154.
Tamar Kennet-Cohen Dvir Kleper Elliot Turvall 《The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology》2018,71(1):39-59
A frequent topic of psychological research is the estimation of the correlation between two variables from a sample that underwent a selection process based on a third variable. Due to indirect range restriction, the sample correlation is a biased estimator of the population correlation, and a correction formula is used. In the past, bootstrap standard error and confidence intervals for the corrected correlations were examined with normal data. The present study proposes a large-sample estimate (an analytic method) for the standard error, and a corresponding confidence interval for the corrected correlation. Monte Carlo simulation studies involving both normal and non-normal data were conducted to examine the empirical performance of the bootstrap and analytic methods. Results indicated that with both normal and non-normal data, the bootstrap standard error and confidence interval were generally accurate across simulation conditions (restricted sample size, selection ratio, and population correlations) and outperformed estimates of the analytic method. However, with certain combinations of distribution type and model conditions, the analytic method has an advantage, offering reasonable estimates of the standard error and confidence interval without resorting to the bootstrap procedure's computer-intensive approach. We provide SAS code for the simulation studies. 相似文献
155.
Mohsen Joshanloo 《The Japanese psychological research》2018,60(3):162-169
This study investigated the factor structure of the 26‐item Midlife Development Inventory (MIDI) Personality Scale in a sample of 2,720 Americans. It was found that whereas confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) did not provide an acceptable fit to the data, exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM) provided an acceptable fit. The results of ESEM revealed that the a priori five‐factor structure of personality was generally consistent with the data, and all items had salient loadings on their target factors. ESEM also revealed that some of the items contributed significantly to more than one personality factor. The results are in line with previous research, and indicate that ESEM is more suitable than CFA for the study of personality traits. 相似文献
156.
157.
Samuel S. Nordberg Andrew A. McAleavey Elizabeth Duszak Benjamin D. Locke Jeffrey A. Hayes Louis G. Castonguay 《Counselling psychology quarterly》2018,31(1):25-41
Objective: The authors attempted to develop and validate a general distress index for a multidimensional psychological symptom/outcome measure used in over 300 college counseling centers with more than 100,000 cases annually: the Counseling Center Assessment of Psychological Symptoms (CCAPS). Method: Four models were compared for fit indices (n = 19,247): the existing first-order factor model (without a general factor), a second-order factor model, a bifactor model, and a single factor or “total score” model. In separate clinical and non-clinical samples, concurrent and divergent validity were examined using several well-established measures of psychological symptoms, as well as two-week test–retest and treatment utilization data. Results: Second-order and bifactor models which captured a single “distress” factor both exhibited good fit to the data relative to the baseline and “total score” model. Validity data indicated that factors adequately measured meaningful clinical onstructs. Conclusion: Both the bifactor and second-order models indicated the presence of a “distress index” comprised items across many of the CCAPS subscales. This distress scale has strong applicability for benchmarking the overall severity and complexity of patients at different centers, and can be used to help identify colleges and universities with areas of clinical strength, which can be studied to improve the field. Clinically, the distress index offers a parsimonious and efficient method for clinicians to monitor patients’ progress through treatment. 相似文献
158.
Davide Serpico 《Philosophical Psychology》2018,31(2):232-252
The model of human intelligence that is most widely adopted derives from psychometrics and behavioral genetics. This standard approach conceives intelligence as a general cognitive ability that is genetically highly heritable and describable using quantitative traits analysis. The paper analyzes intelligence within the debate on natural kinds and contends that the general intelligence conceptualization does not carve psychological nature at its joints. Moreover, I argue that this model assumes an essentialist perspective. As an alternative, I consider an HPC theory of intelligence and evaluate how it deals with essentialism and with intuitions coming from cognitive science. Finally, I highlight some concerns about the HPC model as well, and conclude by suggesting that it is unnecessary to treat intelligence as a kind in any sense. 相似文献
159.
This study examined the underlying factor structure of 15 narrative meaning-making indices for narratives of stressful events, and explored the incremental validity of the narrative factor solution over and above general personality traits in predicting various indices of psychological well-being. Two-hundred and twenty four undergraduates (Mage?=?19.2 years, SDage?=?2.1; 114 males and 110 females; 67.6% Caucasian, 12.0% East Asian, 7.6% African-American, 4.0% South Asian, 2.2% Hispanic, and 6.7% as mixed or Other origin) wrote about the most traumatic experience in their life, and completed a series of psychological questionnaires. The narratives were coded in 15 ways theoretically derived from the narrative meaning-making literature. A series of exploratory structural equation models indicated that a four-factor solution best approximated the data. The four factors were: positive processing, negative processing, integrative meaning, and structure. All four factors related differentially to indices of well-being over and above traits. There appear to be four distinct, but related, factors of narrative meaning-making for memories of stressful events, which shed light on the nuanced relations with well-being. 相似文献
160.
Jesse Fox W. Bryce Hagedorn Stephen A. Sivo 《Counselling and Psychotherapy Research》2016,16(4):244-255